Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where James J. Foster is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by James J. Foster.


Biology Letters | 2015

Spectral information as an orientation cue in dung beetles

Basil el Jundi; James J. Foster; J Byrne Marcus; Emily Baird

During the day, a non-uniform distribution of long and short wavelength light generates a colour gradient across the sky. This gradient could be used as a compass cue, particularly by animals such as dung beetles that rely primarily on celestial cues for orientation. Here, we tested if dung beetles can use spectral cues for orientation by presenting them with monochromatic (green and UV) light spots in an indoor arena. Beetles kept their original bearing when presented with a single light cue, green or UV, or when presented with both light cues set 180° apart. When either the UV or the green light was turned off after the beetles had set their bearing in the presence of both cues, they were still able to maintain their original bearing to the remaining light. However, if the beetles were presented with two identical green light spots set 180° apart, their ability to maintain their original bearing was impaired. In summary, our data show that ball-rolling beetles could potentially use the celestial chromatic gradient as a reference for orientation.


bioRxiv | 2018

Polarisation vision: overcoming challenges of working with a property of light we barely see

James J. Foster; Shelby E. Temple; Martin J. How; Ilse M. Daly; Camilla R. Sharkey; David Wilby; Nicholas W. Roberts

In recent years, the study of polarisation vision in animals has seen numerous breakthroughs, not just in terms of what is known about the function of this sensory ability, but also in the experimental methods by which polarisation can be controlled, presented and measured. Once thought to be limited to only a few animal species, polarisation sensitivity is now known to be widespread across many taxonomic groups, and advances in experimental techniques are, in part, responsible for these discoveries. Nevertheless, its study remains challenging, perhaps because of our own poor sensitivity to the polarisation of light, but equally as a result of the slow spread of new practices and methodological innovations within the field. In this review, we introduce the most important steps in designing and calibrating polarised stimuli, within the broader context of areas of current research and the applications of new techniques to key questions. Our aim is to provide a constructive guide to help researchers, particularly those with no background in the physics of polarisation, to design robust experiments that are free from confounding factors.


Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences; 285(1871), no 20172322 (2018) | 2018

How animals follow the stars

James J. Foster; Jochen Smolka; Dan Eric Nilsson; Marie Dacke

Throughout history, the stars have provided humans with ever more information about our world, enabling increasingly accurate systems of navigation in addition to fuelling some of the greatest scientific controversies. What information animals have evolved to extract from a starry sky and how they do so, is a topic of study that combines the practical and theoretical challenges faced by both astronomers and field biologists. While a number of animal species have been demonstrated to use the stars as a source of directional information, the strategies that these animals use to convert this complex and variable pattern of dim-light points into a reliable ‘stellar orientation’ cue have been more difficult to ascertain. In this review, we assess the stars as a visual stimulus that conveys directional information, and compare the bodies of evidence available for the different stellar orientation strategies proposed to date. In this context, we also introduce new technologies that may aid in the study of stellar orientation, and suggest how field experiments may be used to characterize the mechanisms underlying stellar orientation.


bioRxiv | 2018

Orienting to Polarized Light at Night-Matching Lunar Skylight to Performance in a Nocturnal Beetle

James J. Foster; John Daniel Kirwan; Basil el Jundi; Jochen Smolka; Lana Khaldy; Emily Baird; Marcus J. Byrne; Dan-E Nilsson; Sönke Johnsen; Marie Dacke

For polarized light to inform behaviour, the typical range of degrees of polarization observable in the animal’s natural environment must be above the threshold for detection and interpretation. Here we present the first investigation of the degree of linear polarization threshold for orientation behaviour in a nocturnal species, with specific reference to the range of degrees of polarization measured in the night sky. An effect of lunar phase on the degree of polarization of skylight was found, with smaller illuminated fractions of the moon’s surface corresponding to lower degrees of polarization in the night sky. We found that South African dung beetle Escarabaeus satyrus (Boheman, 1860) can orient to polarized light for a range of degrees of polarization similar to that observed in diurnal insects, reaching a lower threshold between 0.04 and 0.32, possibly as low as 0.11. For degrees of polarization lower than 0.23, as measured on a crescent moon night, orientation performance was considerably weaker than that observed for completely linearly-polarized stimuli, but was nonetheless stronger than in the absence of polarized light. Summary Statement A degree-of-polarization threshold for orientation behaviour is reported for nocturnal dung beetle Escarabaeus satyrus in the context of measurements showing changes in the degree of polarization of skylight with lunar phase.


bioRxiv | 2018

The roles of vision and antennal mechanoreception in hawkmoth flight control.

Ajinkya Dahake; Anna Stoeckl; James J. Foster; Sanjay P. Sane; Almut Kelber

Flying animals need constant sensory feedback about their body position and orientation for flight control. The visual system provides valuable but slow feedback. In contrast, mechanosensory channels can provide feedback at much shorter response times. How the contributions from the two senses are integrated is still an open question in most insect groups. In Diptera, fast mechanosensory feedback is provided by organs called halteres, and is crucial for the control of rapid flight manoeuvres, while vision controls manoeuvres in lower temporal frequency bands. Here we have investigated the visual-mechanosensory integration in an insect which lacks halteres: the hawkmoth Macroglossum stellatarum. It is representative for a large group of insects that use mechanoreceptive Johnstons organs in their antennae to provide gyroscopic feedback on perturbations in body position. High-speed videos of freely-flying hawkmoths hovering at stationary or oscillating artificial flowers show that positional fidelity during flight was reduced in antennectomised animals, but was rescued after antennal re-attachment. Our experiments show that antennal mechanosensory feedback specifically supports fast flight manoeuvres (flower oscillations between 2-6 Hz), but not slow ones. Differences in the reliability of visual feedback (in different light intensities) affected all antennal conditions equally, suggesting there was no compensatory interaction between antennal and visual feedback under the conditions tested. These results establish the importance of antennal mechanosensors in providing rapid mechanosensory feedback for finer control of flight manoeuvres, acting in parallel to visual feedback.Flying animals need constant sensory feedback about their body position and orientation for flight control. The visual system provides essential but slow feedback. In contrast, mechanosensory channels can provide feedback at much shorter timescales. How the contributions from these two senses are integrated remains an open question in most insect groups. In Diptera, fast mechanosensory feedback is provided by organs called halteres, and is crucial for the control of rapid flight manoeuvres, while vision controls manoeuvres in lower temporal frequency bands. Here we have investigated the visual-mechanosensory integration in an insect which lacks halteres: the hawkmoth Macroglossum stellatarum. They represent a large group of insects that use Johnston’s organs in their antennae to provide mechanosensory feedback on perturbations in body position. High-speed videos of freely-flying hawkmoths hovering at stationary or oscillating artificial flowers showed that positional fidelity during flight was reduced in flagella ablated animals, but was recovered after flagella re-attachment. Our experiments show that antennal mechanosensory feedback specifically mediates fast flight manoeuvres, but not slow ones. Differences in the latency of visual feedback (in different light intensities) affected all antennal conditions equally, suggesting there was no compensatory interaction between antennal and visual feedback under the tested conditions. These results establish the importance of antennal mechanosensors in providing rapid mechanosensory feedback for finer control of flight manoeuvres, acting in parallel to visual feedback.


The Science of Nature | 2018

Correction to: Polarisation vision: overcoming challenges of working with a property of light we barely see

James J. Foster; Shelby E. Temple; Martin J. How; Ilse M. Daly; Camilla R. Sharkey; David Wilby; Nicholas W. Roberts

In “Polarisation vision: overcoming challenges of working with a property of light we barely see” (Foster et al. 2018) we provide a basic description of how Stokes parameters can be estimated and used to calculate the angle of polarisation (AoP).


The Journal of Experimental Biology | 2018

The sea urchin Diadema africanum uses low resolution vision to find shelter and deter enemies

John Daniel Kirwan; Jochen Smolka; James J. Foster; José Carlos Hernández; Dan-Eric Nilsson

ABSTRACT Many sea urchins can detect light on their body surface and some species are reported to possess image-resolving vision. Here, we measure the spatial resolution of vision in the long-spined sea urchin Diadema africanum, using two different visual responses: a taxis towards dark objects and an alarm response of spine-pointing towards looming stimuli. For the taxis response we used visual stimuli, which were isoluminant to the background, to discriminate spatial vision from phototaxis. Individual animals were placed in the centre of a cylindrical arena under bright down-welling light, with stimuli of varying angular width placed on the arena wall at alternating directions from the centre. We tracked the direction of movement of individual animals in relation to the stimuli to determine whether the animals oriented towards the stimulus. We found that D. africanum responds by taxis towards isoluminant stimuli with a spatial resolution in the range of 29–69 deg. This corresponds to a theoretical acceptance angle of 38–89 deg, assuming a contrast threshold of 10%. The visual acuity of the alarm response of D. africanum was tested by exposing animals to different sized dark looming and appearing stimuli on a monitor. We found that D. africanum displays a spine-pointing response to appearing black circles of 13–25 deg angular width, corresponding to an acceptance angle of 60–116 deg, assuming the same contrast threshold as above. Summary: Using a combination of imaging and behavioural experiments, testing visual detection via two photic behaviours, the authors show that the sea urchin Diadema africanum has low resolution vision.


The Journal of Experimental Biology | 2018

Hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) use visual contrast in self-assessment of camouflage

David Wilby; Samuel Riches; Ilse M. Daly; Andrew Bird; Matthew Wheelwright; James J. Foster

ABSTRACT Animals can make use of camouflage to reduce the likelihood of visual detection or recognition and thus improve their chances of survival. Background matching, where body colouration is closely matched to the surrounding substrate, is one form of camouflage. Hermit crabs have the opportunity to choose their camouflage independently of body colouration as they inhabit empty gastropod shells, making them ideal to study their choice of camouflage. We used 3D-printed artificial shells of varying contrasts against a grey substrate to test whether hermit crabs prefer shells that they perceive as less conspicuous. Contrast-minimising shells were chosen for Weber contrasts stronger than −0.5. However, in looming experiments, animals responded to contrasts as weak as −0.2, indicating that while they can detect differences between shells and the background, they are only motivated to move into those shells when the alternatives contrast strongly. This suggests a trade-off between camouflage and vulnerability introduced by switching shells. Summary: Hermit crabs prefer shells that are matched to the brightness of their background but make a trade-off between the benefits of camouflaged shells and the danger associated with switching shells.


Current Biology | 2016

A Snapshot-Based Mechanism for Celestial Orientation

Basil el Jundi; James J. Foster; Lana Khaldy; Marcus J. Byrne; Marie Dacke; Emily Baird


Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B | 2017

Stellar performance: Mechanisms underlying milky way orientation in dung beetles

James J. Foster; Basil el Jundi; Jochen Smolka; Lana Khaldy; Dan Eric Nilsson; Marcus J. Byrne; Marie Dacke

Collaboration


Dive into the James J. Foster's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marcus J. Byrne

University of the Witwatersrand

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge