James S. Ferraro
Binghamton University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by James S. Ferraro.
Physiology & Behavior | 1998
Laura L. Murphy; Rhonda S. Cadena; Dan Chávez; James S. Ferraro
The effects of American ginseng (Panax quinquefolium) on male rat copulatory behavior were investigated. Adult Sprague-Dawley rats were administered either 10, 50 or 100 mg/kg of Panax quinquefolium or a sesame oil vehicle per os (p.o.) for 28 days and copulatory behavior parameters were measured. Ginseng-treated male rats demonstrated a significant decrease in mount, intromission and ejaculation latencies compared to vehicle controls. Hormone analyses revealed no difference in plasma luteinizing hormone or testosterone levels between ginseng- and vehicle-treated animals; however, plasma prolactin levels were significantly reduced by all doses of ginseng tested. When male rats were treated with the 100 mg/kg dose of ginseng for 1, 14 or 28 days, mount and intromission latencies were significantly reduced at 14 and 28 days of daily ginseng treatment, whereas ejaculation latency was significantly reduced after 1 day of ginseng treatment when compared to vehicle controls. Plasma prolactin levels were also significantly decreased after 14 and 28 days of daily ginseng administration. There were no differences in body weight or in testes, seminal vesicle, anterior pituitary or spleen weights between ginseng- and vehicle-treated rats. These results demonstrate that P. quinquefolium significantly facilitates male copulatory behavior. The reduction in plasma prolactin levels suggests that ginseng-induced alterations in dopaminergic neurotransmission may play a role in the ability of P. quinquefolium to stimulate copulatory behavior in the male rat.
Physiology & Behavior | 1992
Frank M. Sulzman; James S. Ferraro; Charles A. Fuller; Martin C. Moore-Ede; V. Klimovitsky; V. Magedov; A.M. Alpatov
This study examines the activity, axillary temperature (T(ax)), and ankle skin temperature (Tsk) of two male Rhesus monkeys exposed to microgravity in space. The animals were flown on a Soviet biosatellite mission (COSMOS 1514). Measurements on the flight animals, as well as synchronous flight controls, were performed in the Soviet Union. Additional control studies were performed in the United States to examine the possible role of metabolic heat production in the T(ax) response observed during the spaceflight. All monkeys were exposed to a 24-h light-dark cycle (LD 16:8) throughout these studies. During weightlessness, T(ax) in both flight animals was lower than on earth. The largest difference (0.75 degree C) occurred during the night. There was a reduction in mean heart rate and Tsk during flight. This suggests a reduction in both heat loss and metabolic rate during spaceflight. Although the circadian rhythms in all variables were present during flight, some differences were noted. For example, the amplitude of the rhythms in Tsk and activity were attenuated. Furthermore, the T(ax) and activity rhythms did not have precise 24.0 hour periods and may have been externally desynchronized from the 24-h LD cycle. These data suggest a weakening of the coupling between the internal circadian pacemaker and the external LD synchronizer.
Physiology & Behavior | 1990
A.J. Antipas; D.M. Madison; James S. Ferraro
Circadian rhythms of wheel running and feeding were measured in the short-tailed shrew. Shrews were strongly nocturnal, and their activity rhythms entrained to both long-day (LD 16:8) and short-day (LD 6:18) photocycles. Under conditions of continuous light (LL) or darkness (DD), the activity rhythms free-ran with average periodicities of 25.1 hours and 24.1 hours, respectively. In LL the level of activity was depressed, and in some cases wheel running was completely inhibited. No significant sex differences were observed in the period or amplitude of the monitored circadian rhythms. All shrews fed throughout the day and night; however, unlike in previous reports, ultradian periods of feeding behavior were not found. The results are related to Aschoffs four observations for the effect of light on activity rhythms in nocturnal rodents.
Physiology & Behavior | 1986
James S. Ferraro; Charles E. McCormack
To further explore the validity of the non-parametric model of entrainment for predicting the phase-shifting effects of light pulses, we exposed rats to several intensities of continuous light (LL) and feedback lighting (LDFB). LDFB is a lighting condition that exposes the animal to light only during the interval of active locomotion; this interval is coincident with the photosensitive portion of the circadian cycle as defined by the phase-response-curve. This is achieved by linking lights on with locomotor activity. In addition to the comparison of LL with LDFB, the duration of the LDFB pulse was also varied in four rats. Whether rats were exposed to LL or LDFB, as light intensity increased, the free-running period (tau) of the locomotor activity rhythm also increased. This intensity-related increase in tau, which is known as the Aschoff effect, was similar for LL and LDFB 2 min at each light intensity (0.1, 1, and 100 lux). However, when the LDFB pulses were shortened from a duration of 2 min to a duration of approximately 1 sec, tau shortened significantly. These results demonstrate that the non-parametric model of entrainment adequately explains the major period-lengthening effects of LL. However, there are temporal limits to the light pulses that can be used to simulate the effects of LL (i.e., one second light pulses fail to produce the effects brought about by longer pulses).
Biological Rhythm Research | 1988
James S. Ferraro
The non-parametric model of entrainment suggests that brief pulses of light, delivered between dusk and dawn can simulate the phasing effects of full photoperiods or even constant light (LL). Feedback lighting (LDFB) is a lighting condition where individual animals, otherwise in constant darkness (DD), are exposed to light in response to a monitored behavior. The specific purpose of this type of illumination is to expose the circadian cycle to light only during the subjective night. LDFB has been used to support this hypothesis in several species of nocturnal rodents and one species of diurnal primate by producing similar free-running periods in LDFB as in LL. This lighting condition has also been used to test the hypothesis that exposing the subjective night to even short duration light pulses will maintain reproductive function in long day breeders. In the Syrian hamster (Mesocricetus auratus), however, LDFB is not as photostimulatory as LL despite extensive light exposure during the subjective night. In the experiments presented here, a group of immature male Djungarian hamsters (Phodopus sungorus) were placed in individual light-tight sound attenuated chambers where they had free access to food, water and an activity wheel. The animals were exposed to one of four lighting conditions [DD, LL, LDFB or a neighbor control of feedback lighting (LDFB NC)] for approximately 30 days shortly after weaning. LDFB NC is a lighting condition where a neighbor control hamster receives the identical lighting regime as a paired animal exposing itself to LDFB, yet the neighbor has no control over it. A fifth group was exposed to a light-dark cycle of 16 hours of light and 8 hours of dark (LD16:8). This group was housed in cages in a colony room and did not have access to a running wheel. The free-running periods of the locomotor activity rhythms for hamsters exposed to LDFB and LL were not similar, unlike the results for rats, Syrian hamsters, mice, monkeys and even mature Djungarian hamsters. Immature hamsters exposed to DD and LDFB NC developed more slowly than animals exposed to LL or LD16:8, while hamsters in LDFB developed at an intermediate rate. Thus, it appears that LDFB, although capable of inducing reproductive function in immature Djungarian hamsters, is not as photostimulatory as may have been expected from current photoperiodic models, despite substantial light exposure during the subjective night. Furthermore, this data may suggest that the circadian system of 18-48 day old Djungarian hamsters are still undergoing organizational maturation.
Biological Rhythm Research | 1994
James S. Ferraro; J. A. Dorsett; Thomas E. Wagner; Jun S. Yun; Andrzej Bartke
Abstract Transgenic mice were produced by microinjection of male pronuclei with approximately 2.7Kb DNA fragment, containing a metallothionein‐I promoter (MT) or a phosphoenolpyruvate carboxykinase (PEPCK) promoter linked to human growth hormone (hGH) or bovine growth hormone (bGH) structural genes. Transgenic mice from resulting lines have substantial levels of circulating heterologous GH and are much larger than normal mice. Since these animals have reproductive abnormalities, and since reproductive hormones have significant effects on the circadian timing system, experiments were designed to determine whether these animals had altered freerunning periods. Transgenic female mice and their normal female siblings were individually housed in cages with activity wheels and exposed to constant dark (DD) or constant light (LL) for durations exceeding two weeks. Locomotor activity was continuously monitored by computer. The period of the circadian rhythm of locomotor activity and the duration of activity were ...
Advances in Space Research | 1989
James S. Ferraro; Charles A. Fuller; Frank M. Sulzman
The circadian rhythm of conidiation in Neurospora crassa is thought to be an endogenously derived circadian oscillation; however, several investigators have suggested that circadian rhythms may, instead, be driven by some geophysical time cue(s). An experiment was conducted on space shuttle flight STS-9 in order to test this hypothesis; during the first 7-8 cycles in space, there were several minor alterations observed in the conidiation rhythm, including an increase in the period of the oscillation, an increase in the variability of the growth rate and a diminished rhythm amplitude, which eventually damped out in 25% of the flight tubes. On day seven of flight, the tubes were exposed to light while their growth fronts were marked. Some aspect of the marking process reinstated a robust rhythm in all the tubes which continued throughout the remainder of the flight. These results from the last 86 hours of flight demonstrated that the rhythm can persist in space. Since the aberrant rhythmicity occurred prior to the marking procedure, but not after, it was hypothesized that the damping on STS-9 may have resulted from the hypergravity pulse of launch. To test this hypothesis, we conducted investigations into the effects of altered gravitational forces on conidiation. Exposure to hypergravity (via centrifugation), simulated microgravity (via the use of a clinostat) and altered orientations (via alterations in the vector of a 1 g force) were used to examine the effects of gravity upon the circadian rhythm of conidiation.
Physiology & Behavior | 1984
James S. Ferraro; W. Stone; Charles E. McCormack
This paper describes an electronic device through which environmental lighting conditions are linked to locomotor activity thus allowing only the photosensitive portions of a nocturnal rodents phase-response-curve to be exposed to light. In the past, this type of lighting schedule has been difficult, if not impossible, to present with an exogenously controlled lighting system due to the phase shifting ability of the rodents circadian system. The feedback lighting system is made from components which can be purchased at most electronics outlets for less than
Physiology & Behavior | 1990
James S. Ferraro; H.N. Krum; Andrzej Bartke; G.T. Wassmer; V. Chandrashekar; Sandra D. Michael; F.M. Sulzman
100.
Archive | 2009
Laura L. Murphy; James S. Ferraro
In an effort to determine the inductive component(s) of photic input in long day seasonal breeders, adult male Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) were exposed to one of nine lighting conditions for a duration of 10 weeks: a light-dark cycle of 14 hours of light followed by 10 hours of dark (LD 14:10, a long photoperiod); LD 10:14 (a short photoperiod); a high frequency light-dark cycle of 1 hour of light and 1 hour of dark (LD 1:1); a higher frequency light-dark cycle of 1 minute of light and 1 minute of dark (LD 1m:1m); constant light (LL); constant dark (DD); feedback lighting (LDFB; a condition that illuminates the cage in response to locomotor activity); a feedback lighting neighbor control (LDFB NC; the animal receives the same light pattern as a paired animal in LDFB, but has no control over it); or reverse feedback lighting (rLDFB; a condition that darkens an illuminated cage in response to locomotor activity). Exposure to LL, LD 1:1, LD 1m:1m, LDFB and rLDFB significantly and similarly lengthened the free-running period of the locomotor rhythm when compared to the period of animals in DD. The paired tests and accessory reproductive glands weights, spermiogenesis, seminiferous tubule diameter and serum concentrations of testosterone, prolactin, LH and FSH, suggest that LD 14:10, LL, LD 1:1, rLDFB and LDFB NC maintain reproductive function in the Syrian hamster, while LD 10:14, DD, LD 1m:1m and LDFB do not. It is known that as little as two 1-second pulses of light are stimulatory if coincident with the subjective night (17.22). Thus, it is not surprising that LD 1:1 is stimulatory. LD 1m:1m is not stimulatory, however, despite an identical quanta of light per 24 hours and similar phase relationship. It appears that mere light exposure during the subjective night is not necessarily reproductively inductive. It would also appear that behaviorally generated light-dark cycles can be (i.e., LDFB), but are not necessarily (i.e., rLDFB) inhibitory to the maintenance of the reproductive system in long day breeders. Furthermore, the lighting pattern derived from LDFB is stimulatory if given exogenously (i.e., LDFB NC). Although it is not understood why light exposure that is coincident with the subjective night (i.e., LD 1m:1m and LDFB) is not stimulatory in long day breeders, a possible hypothesis is that an internal coincidence model is involved in the photoperiodic response and that multiple transitions during the subjective night may cause a dissociation of internal oscillations which must be in phase for light to be stimulatory.