James Slotta
University of California, San Diego
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by James Slotta.
Cognitive Science | 2018
Kensy Cooperrider; James Slotta; Rafael Núñez
Pointing is a cornerstone of human communication, but does it take the same form in all cultures? Manual pointing with the index finger appears to be used universally, and it is often assumed to be universally preferred over other forms. Non-manual pointing with the head and face has also been widely attested, but it is usually considered of marginal significance, both empirically and theoretically. Here, we challenge this assumed marginality. Using a novel communication task, we investigated pointing preferences in the Yupno of Papua New Guinea and in U.S. undergraduates. Speakers in both groups pointed at similar rates, but form preferences differed starkly: The Yupno participants used non-manual pointing (nose- and head-pointing) numerically more often than manual pointing, whereas the U.S. participants stuck unwaveringly to index-finger pointing. The findings raise questions about why groups differ in their pointing preferences and, more broadly, about why humans communicate in the ways they do.
Archive | 2016
James Slotta
Efforts to demarcate what slang is tend to dwell on pragmatics—that is, the relationship of slangy speech to the context in which it is used as, variously: a way of indicating something about its user’s identity, a mode of fostering in-group solidarity among interactional participants, a mark of the “informality” of the speech event, and so on. So, for example, Eble defines slang as “an ever changing set of colloquial words and phrases that speakers use to establish or reinforce social identity or cohesiveness within a group or with a trend or fashion in society at large” (1996, p. 11). Dumas and Lighter find the common denominator of slang lexemes to be “their undeniable lack of dignity and their deliberate, widespread use within a social group...to defy social or linguistic convention” (1978, p. 16). Among Spears’ list of 10 characteristic features of slang, we find many of the same attributes: “1. Slang is not considered suitable for formal or serious matters; 2. Slang terms are usually synonymous for standard terms; 3. Slang terms and slang speech symbolize a lack of allegiance to social conventions...” (1981, p. viii).
Cognitive Science | 2014
Kensy Cooperrider; Rafael Núñez; James Slotta
The Protean Pointing Gesture: Variation in a Building Block of Human Communication Kensy Cooperrider ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, University of Chicago 5848 S. University Ave., Chicago, IL 60637 Rafael Nunez ([email protected]) Department of Cognitive Science, University of California – San Diego 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, CA 92093 James Slotta ([email protected]) Department of Anthropology, University of California – San Diego 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, CA 92093 Abstract on these questions. In the folk theories of the English- speaking world and other so-called WEIRD (Western Educated Industrialized Rich Democratic) societies, there is a strong association between pointing and extension of the forefinger, an association enshrined in the English colloquial terms “index” or “pointer” finger. This forefinger-pointing association is not just a matter of terminology. A recent study of infant gestures in seven far- flung speech communities found in each one a preference for index finger pointing over other kinds of manual or non- manual pointing (Liszkowski et al., 2012). Use of the index finger as a kind of developmental pointing default may in fact have its roots in the anatomy of the human hand. When the hand is at rest, the index finger protrudes relative to the other fingers in human children and adults but not in chimpanzees (Povinelli et al., 1994). And, indeed, notwithstanding impressionistic claims made about remote cultures, use of the index finger for pointing has been found in every community in which it has been sought. These facts taken together suggest that index finger pointing may be a strong candidate for a human universal. Countering this swelling tide of evidence for the extended index finger as a species-wide privileged form of the gesture, however, are a handful of reports of substantive cultural differences in how humans point. In several groups, variations in pointing handshape, such as the flat or horned hand, carry conventional meanings (Wilkins, 2003; Kendon & Versante, 2003). Other studies have described conventionalized ways of pointing non-manually, such as by protruding the lips (Sherzer, 1972; Enfield, 2001) or by scrunching the nose (Cooperrider & Nunez, 2012) while directing one’s gaze to a region of space. Though largely absent from WEIRD communities, such non-manual pointing practices may not be so rare. Lip-pointing in one variant or another appears to be widely distributed, with reports of its use in Southeast Asia, Australia, the Caribbean, Africa, and South America. Should such differences of form be considered surface- level cultural quirks, or do they imply that this foundational building block is more protean than often assumed? Existing Pointing is a foundational building block of human communication, but does it take the same form from one culture to the next? Index finger pointing is often assumed to be universally privileged. Use of non-manual pointing morphologies has been attested around the world but it has never been clear how central these variants are in the communities in which they occur. Using a novel referential communication task, we investigated pointing preferences in two cultures: in the Yupno of Papua New Guinea and in the US. Our task prompted similar rates of pointing in both groups, but the Yupno participants produced more non- manual pointing (nose- and head-pointing) than manual pointing, while the US participants stuck unwaveringly to index finger pointing. The motivation for these starkly contrasting patterns requires further investigation, but it is clear they constitute fundamentally different ways of carrying out one of our most distinctively human communicative acts. Keywords: pointing; reference; communicative universals; human diversity; embodiment; Papua New Guinea Introduction Humans have been influentially described as the “symbolic species” but might just as well be described as the “deictic species.” Evidence of animal pointing in the wild is scarce and contested, but human infants everywhere, sometime before they can speak, start to point for their caretakers. This prelinguistic gesture, an early bodily expression of the drive to orient and share the attention of others, remains a basic communicative tool throughout the lifespan and is deployed in all kinds of everyday activities (Clark, 1996). Such observations have led some researchers to posit a special role for pointing at the dawn of human language (e.g. Tomasello, 2008). Primordial or not, there is little question that pointing constitutes, in the words of one researcher, a “foundational building block of human communication” (Kita, 2003). What form does this building block take and is it the same form from one culture to the next? Despite decades of philosophical and cognitive scientific interest in pointing, there has been surprisingly little systematic empirical work
Comparative Studies in Society and History | 2015
James Slotta
Language & Communication | 2012
James Slotta
American Ethnologist | 2017
James Slotta
American Ethnologist | 2017
James Slotta
Language in Society | 2015
James Slotta
Cognitive Science | 2017
Kensy Cooperrider; James Slotta; Rafael Núñez
Journal of Sociolinguistics | 2018
Luke Fleming; James Slotta