Jamie D. Barrett
University of Oklahoma
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Jamie D. Barrett.
Creativity and Innovation Management | 2009
Cristina L. Byrne; Michael J. Mumford; Jamie D. Barrett; William B. Vessey
Due to the rapidly changing economy, innovation is becoming more and more critical for the success and survival of many organizations. Although several factors shaping innovation (e.g., strategy, climate, etc.) have been examined in the literature, the leadership of innovation has received relatively less attention. In this article, we attempt to summarize the literature on the leadership of creative efforts focusing on critical leadership behaviours. We also explore a model of core leader functions tailored for creativity and innovation, as well as a model of innovation planning. The implications of these models for the leadership of innovation are discussed.
Creativity Research Journal | 2012
Michael D. Mumford; Kimberly S. Hester; Issac C. Robledo; David R. Peterson; Eric Anthony Day; Dean F. Hougen; Jamie D. Barrett
Knowledge, or expertise, has been held to contribute to creative problem-solving. In this effort, the relationship of one form of knowledge, mental models, to creative problem-solving was assessed. Undergraduates were asked to solve either a marketing or an education problem calling for creative thought. Prior to generating solutions to these problems, the mental models used by undergraduates to understand problems in these domains were assessed in terms of their objective and subjective features. It was found that both objective and subjective features of peoples mental models were related to the quality, originality, and elegance of problem solutions. The implications of these findings for understanding the role of mental models in creative problem-solving are discussed.
Creativity Research Journal | 2012
Issac C. Robledo; Kimberly S. Hester; David R. Peterson; Jamie D. Barrett; Eric Anthony Day; Dean P. Hougen; Michael D. Mumford
People make errors in their creative problem-solving efforts. The intent of this article was to assess whether error-management training would improve performance on creative problem-solving tasks. Undergraduates were asked to solve an educational leadership problem known to call for creative thought where problem solutions were scored for quality, originality, and elegance. Prior to beginning work on their problem solutions, participants were provided with training in 0 to 4 error-management strategies. It was found that error-management training was beneficial for talented people (as indicated by scores on the pretraining exercise) resulting in solutions of greater originality. The implications of these findings for improving performance on creative problem-solving tasks are discussed.
Creativity Research Journal | 2012
Kimberly S. Hester; Issac C. Robledo; Jamie D. Barrett; David R. Peterson; Dean P. Hougen; Eric Anthony Day; Michael D. Mumford
In recent years, it has become apparent that knowledge is a critical component of creative thought. One form of knowledge that might be particularly important to creative thought relies on the mental models people employ to understand novel, ill-defined problems. In this study, undergraduates were given training in the use of causal relationships in applying mental models in creative problem-solving. A pre-post design was used to assess the effects of this training on mental models and creative problem-solving. It was found that causal analysis training resulted in the acquisition of better mental models (in terms of subjective and objective attributes) and better solutions (in terms of quality, originality, and elegance) to problems calling for creative thought among high-ability participants. The implications of these findings for understanding the role of mental models in creative problem-solving are discussed.
Creativity Research Journal | 2013
Jamie D. Barrett; David R. Peterson; Kimberly S. Hester; Issac C. Robledo; Eric Anthony Day; Dean P. Hougen; Michael D. Mumford
Many techniques have been used to train creative problem-solving skills. Although the available techniques have often proven to be effective, creative training often discounts the value of thinking about applications. In this study, 248 undergraduates were asked to develop advertising campaigns for a new high-energy soft drink. Solutions to this problem were evaluated for quality, originality, and elegance. Prior to preparing these advertising campaigns, participants were provided with training in strategies for thinking about the potential applications of creative problem-solutions. It was found that training people to think about the uses of ideas and preparation for idea implementation contributed to the acquisition of stronger mental models and production of advertising campaigns evidencing greater quality, originality, and elegance. The implications of these findings for creative though and creative education are discussed.
Creativity Research Journal | 2013
David R. Peterson; Jamie D. Barrett; Kimberly S. Hester; Issac C. Robledo; Dean F. Hougen; Eric Anthony Day; Michael D. Mumford
Constraints often inhibit creative problem-solving. This study examined the impact of training strategies for managing constraints on creative problem-solving. Undergraduates, 218 in all, were asked to work through 1 to 4 self-paced instructional programs focused on constraint management strategies. The quality, originality, and elegance of solutions to social innovation problems were assessed. It was found that providing training in managing resource constraints was generally valuable. For talented individuals, providing training in managing user skill constraints also proved valuable. The implications of these findings for training intended to improve creative thinking are discussed.
Creativity Research Journal | 2014
Jamie D. Barrett; William B. Vessey; Jennifer A. Griffith; Derek Mracek; Michael D. Mumford
There is little doubt that career experiences contribute to scientific achievement; however this relationship has yet to be thoroughly investigated in terms the effects on scientific creativity. In this study, a historiometric approach was used to examine 3 areas of adult career experiences common to scientific achievement. In doing so, prior theoretical work was used to identify career experiences relevant to scientific achievement, and 3 theoretical models were proposed to account for these experiences—adversity, collaborations, and work strategies. Biographies of eminent scientists were then content coded and analyzed. The results indicated that the adversity model did not predict scientific creativity. However, the work strategies model and, to some degree, the collaborations model showed some promise in understanding the development of creative potential in scientists. The nature of the significant relationships among the model components and scientific creativity are discussed in addition to their implications for the development of the creative potential of scientists.
Leadership Quarterly | 2014
William B. Vessey; Jamie D. Barrett; Michael D. Mumford; Genevieve Johnson; Brett Litwiller
Leadership Quarterly | 2011
William B. Vessey; Jamie D. Barrett; Michael D. Mumford
Leadership Quarterly | 2011
Jamie D. Barrett; William B. Vessey; Michael D. Mumford