Jeannette Haviland-Jones
Rutgers University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Jeannette Haviland-Jones.
Evolutionary Psychology | 2005
Jeannette Haviland-Jones; Holly Hale Rosario; Patricia Wilson; Terry R. McGuire
For more than 5000 years, people have cultivated flowers although there is no known reward for this costly behavior. In three different studies we show that flowers are a powerful positive emotion “inducer”. In Study 1, flowers, upon presentation to women, always elicited the Duchenne or true smile. Women who received flowers reported more positive moods 3 days later. In Study 2, a flower given to men or women in an elevator elicited more positive social behavior than other stimuli. In Study 3, flowers presented to elderly participants (55+ age) elicited positive mood reports and improved episodic memory. Flowers have immediate and long-term effects on emotional reactions, mood, social behaviors and even memory for both males and females. There is little existing theory in any discipline that explains these findings. We suggest that cultivated flowers are rewarding because they have evolved to rapidly induce positive emotion in humans, just as other plants have evolved to induce varying behavioral responses in a wide variety of species leading to the dispersal or propagation of the plants.
PLOS ONE | 2016
Jeannette Haviland-Jones; Terry R. McGuire; Patricia Wilson
Mood odor identification, explicit awareness of mood odor, may be an important emotion skill and part of a complex dual processing system. It has already been shown that mood odors have significant implicit effects, effects that occur without awareness. This study applies methods for examining human individual differences in the identification of chemosignals for fear and happy, important in itself, and a key to understanding the dual processing of emotion in the olfactory system. Axillary mood odors had been collected from 14 male donors during a mood induction task. Pads were collected after 12 and 24 minutes, creating two doses. Sixty -one participants (41 females) identified the mood odor chemosignals. On a single trial, participants identified 2 doses of fear, 2 doses of happy, and a sterile control. There were 15 trials. The first analysis (rtt) showed that the population was phenotypically heterogeneous, not homogeneous, in identification accuracy. It also showed that a minimum of 10 trials was needed for test reliability. The second analysis, Growth Mixture Modeling, found three distinct groups of detectors: (1) 49.49% were consistently accurate super detectors, (2) 32.52% were accurate above chance level detectors, and (3) 17.98% were non-detectors. Bayesian Posterior Analyses showed reliability of groups at or above 98%. No differences related to mood odor valence (fear or happy), dose (collection at 12 or 24 minutes) or gender were found. Implications for further study of genetic differences, learning and function of identification are noted. It appears that many people can be reliable in explicitly identifying fear and happy mood odors but this skill is not homogeneous.
Infant Behavior & Development | 2014
Caroline N. Coffield; Estelle M.Y. Mayhew; Jeannette Haviland-Jones; Arlene S. Walker-Andrews
The effect of odor on cognitive and emotional processes has been studied in adults and children, but less so in infants. In this study twenty-seven six-month-olds were presented with a video while in either an odor (pine or baby-powder) or a no odor control condition. The video was a 92-s audiovisual presentation of a woman expressing happiness and sadness, with the order of emotion counterbalanced. Infant attention (looking time) and emotional expression (smiling, crying, mouthing) were coded. Infants looked longer in the presence of odor and expressed less crying and mouthing but more smiling behavior. Presence of odor markedly reduced infant emotional distress and increased attention, suggesting that the olfactory sensory system provides cues to infants that support mood regulation and maintain attention. These results have implications for optimizing infant environments for emotional health and cognitive development.
Physiology & Behavior | 1999
Denise Chen; Jeannette Haviland-Jones
American Journal of Psychology | 2010
Kenia M. Castellanos; Judith A. Hudson; Jeannette Haviland-Jones; Patricia Wilson
Emotion, Space and Society | 2013
Jeannette Haviland-Jones; Judith A. Hudson; Patricia Wilson; Robin Freyberg; Terry R. McGuire
Behavioral and Brain Sciences | 2005
Arlene S. Walker-Andrews; Jeannette Haviland-Jones
Food Quality and Preference | 2017
Lumeng Jin; Jeannette Haviland-Jones; James E. Simon; Beverly J. Tepper
Archive | 2016
Robin Freyberg; Patricia Wilson; Jeannette Haviland-Jones
Archive | 2016
Jeannette Haviland-Jones; Patricia Wilson; Robin Freyberg