Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Jed T. Elison is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Jed T. Elison.


American Journal of Psychiatry | 2012

Differences in White Matter Fiber Tract Development Present From 6 to 24 Months in Infants With Autism

Jason J. Wolff; Hongbin Gu; Guido Gerig; Jed T. Elison; Martin Styner; Sylvain Gouttard; Kelly N. Botteron; Stephen R. Dager; Geraldine Dawson; Annette Estes; Alan C. Evans; Heather Cody Hazlett; Penelope Kostopoulos; Robert C. McKinstry; Sarah Paterson; Robert T. Schultz; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; Joseph Piven

OBJECTIVE Evidence from prospective studies of high-risk infants suggests that early symptoms of autism usually emerge late in the first or early in the second year of life after a period of relatively typical development. The authors prospectively examined white matter fiber tract organization from 6 to 24 months in high-risk infants who developed autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) by 24 months. METHOD The participants were 92 high-risk infant siblings from an ongoing imaging study of autism. All participants had diffusion tensor imaging at 6 months and behavioral assessments at 24 months; a majority contributed additional imaging data at 12 and/or 24 months. At 24 months, 28 infants met criteria for ASDs and 64 infants did not. Microstructural properties of white matter fiber tracts reported to be associated with ASDs or related behaviors were characterized by fractional anisotropy and radial and axial diffusivity. RESULTS The fractional anisotropy trajectories for 12 of 15 fiber tracts differed significantly between the infants who developed ASDs and those who did not. Development for most fiber tracts in the infants with ASDs was characterized by higher fractional anisotropy values at 6 months followed by slower change over time relative to infants without ASDs. Thus, by 24 months of age, those with ASDs had lower values. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest that aberrant development of white matter pathways may precede the manifestation of autistic symptoms in the first year of life. Longitudinal data are critical to characterizing the dynamic age-related brain and behavior changes underlying this neurodevelopmental disorder.


Nature | 2017

Early brain development in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder

Heather Cody Hazlett; Hongbin Gu; Brent C. Munsell; Sun Hyung Kim; Martin Styner; Jason J. Wolff; Jed T. Elison; Meghan R. Swanson; Hongtu Zhu; Kelly N. Botteron; D. Louis Collins; John N. Constantino; Stephen R. Dager; Annette Estes; Alan C. Evans; Vladimir Fonov; Guido Gerig; Penelope Kostopoulos; Robert C. McKinstry; Juhi Pandey; Sarah Paterson; John R. Pruett; Robert T. Schultz; Dennis W. W. Shaw; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; Joseph Piven

Brain enlargement has been observed in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), but the timing of this phenomenon, and the relationship between ASD and the appearance of behavioural symptoms, are unknown. Retrospective head circumference and longitudinal brain volume studies of two-year olds followed up at four years of age have provided evidence that increased brain volume may emerge early in development. Studies of infants at high familial risk of autism can provide insight into the early development of autism and have shown that characteristic social deficits in ASD emerge during the latter part of the first and in the second year of life. These observations suggest that prospective brain-imaging studies of infants at high familial risk of ASD might identify early postnatal changes in brain volume that occur before an ASD diagnosis. In this prospective neuroimaging study of 106 infants at high familial risk of ASD and 42 low-risk infants, we show that hyperexpansion of the cortical surface area between 6 and 12 months of age precedes brain volume overgrowth observed between 12 and 24 months in 15 high-risk infants who were diagnosed with autism at 24 months. Brain volume overgrowth was linked to the emergence and severity of autistic social deficits. A deep-learning algorithm that primarily uses surface area information from magnetic resonance imaging of the brain of 6–12-month-old individuals predicted the diagnosis of autism in individual high-risk children at 24 months (with a positive predictive value of 81% and a sensitivity of 88%). These findings demonstrate that early brain changes occur during the period in which autistic behaviours are first emerging.


American Journal of Psychiatry | 2013

White Matter Microstructure and Atypical Visual Orienting in 7-Month-Olds at Risk for Autism

Jed T. Elison; Sarah Paterson; Jason J. Wolff; J. Steven Reznick; Noah J. Sasson; Hongbin Gu; Kelly Botteron; Stephen R. Dager; Annette Estes; Alan C. Evans; Guido Gerig; Heather Cody Hazlett; Robert T. Schultz; Martin Styner; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; Joseph Piven

OBJECTIVE The authors sought to determine whether specific patterns of oculomotor functioning and visual orienting characterize 7-month-old infants who later meet criteria for an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and to identify the neural correlates of these behaviors. METHOD Data were collected from 97 infants, of whom 16 were high-familial-risk infants later classified as having an ASD, 40 were high-familial-risk infants who did not later meet ASD criteria (high-risk negative), and 41 were low-risk infants. All infants underwent an eye-tracking task at a mean age of 7 months and a clinical assessment at a mean age of 25 months. Diffusion-weighted imaging data were acquired for 84 of the infants at 7 months. Primary outcome measures included average saccadic reaction time in a visually guided saccade procedure and radial diffusivity (an index of white matter organization) in fiber tracts that included corticospinal pathways and the splenium and genu of the corpus callosum. RESULTS Visual orienting latencies were longer in 7-month-old infants who expressed ASD symptoms at 25 months compared with both high-risk negative infants and low-risk infants. Visual orienting latencies were uniquely associated with the microstructural organization of the splenium of the corpus callosum in low-risk infants, but this association was not apparent in infants later classified as having an ASD. CONCLUSIONS Flexibly and efficiently orienting to salient information in the environment is critical for subsequent cognitive and social-cognitive development. Atypical visual orienting may represent an early prodromal feature of an ASD, and abnormal functional specialization of posterior cortical circuits directly informs a novel model of ASD pathogenesis.


Brain | 2015

Altered corpus callosum morphology associated with autism over the first 2 years of life

Jason J. Wolff; Guido Gerig; John D. Lewis; Takahiro Soda; Martin Styner; Clement Vachet; Kelly N. Botteron; Jed T. Elison; Stephen R. Dager; Annette Estes; Heather Cody Hazlett; Robert T. Schultz; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; Joseph Piven

Numerous brain imaging studies indicate that the corpus callosum is smaller in older children and adults with autism spectrum disorder. However, there are no published studies examining the morphological development of this connective pathway in infants at-risk for the disorder. Magnetic resonance imaging data were collected from 270 infants at high familial risk for autism spectrum disorder and 108 low-risk controls at 6, 12 and 24 months of age, with 83% of infants contributing two or more data points. Fifty-seven children met criteria for ASD based on clinical-best estimate diagnosis at age 2 years. Corpora callosa were measured for area, length and thickness by automated segmentation. We found significantly increased corpus callosum area and thickness in children with autism spectrum disorder starting at 6 months of age. These differences were particularly robust in the anterior corpus callosum at the 6 and 12 month time points. Regression analysis indicated that radial diffusivity in this region, measured by diffusion tensor imaging, inversely predicted thickness. Measures of area and thickness in the first year of life were correlated with repetitive behaviours at age 2 years. In contrast to work from older children and adults, our findings suggest that the corpus callosum may be larger in infants who go on to develop autism spectrum disorder. This result was apparent with or without adjustment for total brain volume. Although we did not see a significant interaction between group and age, cross-sectional data indicated that area and thickness differences diminish by age 2 years. Regression data incorporating diffusion tensor imaging suggest that microstructural properties of callosal white matter, which includes myelination and axon composition, may explain group differences in morphology.


Developmental Science | 2013

Frontolimbic neural circuitry at 6 months predicts individual differences in joint attention at 9 months

Jed T. Elison; Jason J. Wolff; Debra C. Heimer; Sarah J. Paterson; Hongbin Gu; Heather Cody Hazlett; Martin Styner; Guido Gerig; Joseph Piven

Elucidating the neural basis of joint attention in infancy promises to yield important insights into the development of language and social cognition, and directly informs developmental models of autism. We describe a new method for evaluating responding to joint attention performance in infancy that highlights the 9- to 10-month period as a time interval of maximal individual differences. We then demonstrate that fractional anisotropy in the right uncinate fasciculus, a white matter fiber bundle connecting the amygdala to the ventral-medial prefrontal cortex and anterior temporal pole, measured in 6-month-olds predicts individual differences in responding to joint attention at 9 months of age. The white matter microstructure of the right uncinate was not related to receptive language ability at 9 months. These findings suggest that the development of core nonverbal social communication skills in infancy is largely supported by preceding developments within right lateralized frontotemporal brain systems.


Journal of Visualized Experiments | 2012

Eye tracking young children with autism.

Noah J. Sasson; Jed T. Elison

The rise of accessible commercial eye-tracking systems has fueled a rapid increase in their use in psychological and psychiatric research. By providing a direct, detailed and objective measure of gaze behavior, eye-tracking has become a valuable tool for examining abnormal perceptual strategies in clinical populations and has been used to identify disorder-specific characteristics, promote early identification, and inform treatment. In particular, investigators of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have benefited from integrating eye-tracking into their research paradigms. Eye-tracking has largely been used in these studies to reveal mechanisms underlying impaired task performance and abnormal brain functioning, particularly during the processing of social information. While older children and adults with ASD comprise the preponderance of research in this area, eye-tracking may be especially useful for studying young children with the disorder as it offers a non-invasive tool for assessing and quantifying early-emerging developmental abnormalities. Implementing eye-tracking with young children with ASD, however, is associated with a number of unique challenges, including issues with compliant behavior resulting from specific task demands and disorder-related psychosocial considerations. In this protocol, we detail methodological considerations for optimizing research design, data acquisition and psychometric analysis while eye-tracking young children with ASD. The provided recommendations are also designed to be more broadly applicable for eye-tracking children with other developmental disabilities. By offering guidelines for best practices in these areas based upon lessons derived from our own work, we hope to help other investigators make sound research design and analysis choices while avoiding common pitfalls that can compromise data acquisition while eye-tracking young children with ASD or other developmental difficulties.


Science Translational Medicine | 2017

Functional neuroimaging of high-risk 6-month-old infants predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age

Robert W. Emerson; Chloe M. Adams; Tomoyuki Nishino; Heather Cody Hazlett; Jason J. Wolff; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; John N. Constantino; Mark D. Shen; Meghan R. Swanson; Jed T. Elison; Sridhar Kandala; Annette Estes; Kelly N. Botteron; Louis Collins; Stephen R. Dager; Alan C. Evans; Guido Gerig; Hongbin Gu; Robert C. McKinstry; Sarah Paterson; Robert T. Schultz; Martin Styner; Bradley L. Schlaggar; John R. Pruett; Joseph Piven

Functional brain imaging of 6-month-old infants with a high familial risk for autism predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age. Predicting the future with brain imaging In a new study, Emerson et al. show that brain function in infancy can be used to accurately predict which high-risk infants will later receive an autism diagnosis. Using machine learning techniques that identify patterns in the brain’s functional connections, Emerson and colleagues were able to predict with greater than 96% accuracy whether a 6-month-old infant would develop autism at 24 months of age. These findings must be replicated, but they represent an important step toward the early identification of individuals with autism before its characteristic symptoms develop. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by social deficits and repetitive behaviors that typically emerge by 24 months of age. To develop effective early interventions that can potentially ameliorate the defining deficits of ASD and improve long-term outcomes, early detection is essential. Using prospective neuroimaging of 59 6-month-old infants with a high familial risk for ASD, we show that functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging correctly identified which individual children would receive a research clinical best-estimate diagnosis of ASD at 24 months of age. Functional brain connections were defined in 6-month-old infants that correlated with 24-month scores on measures of social behavior, language, motor development, and repetitive behavior, which are all features common to the diagnosis of ASD. A fully cross-validated machine learning algorithm applied at age 6 months had a positive predictive value of 100% [95% confidence interval (CI), 62.9 to 100], correctly predicting 9 of 11 infants who received a diagnosis of ASD at 24 months (sensitivity, 81.8%; 95% CI, 47.8 to 96.8). All 48 6-month-old infants who were not diagnosed with ASD were correctly classified [specificity, 100% (95% CI, 90.8 to 100); negative predictive value, 96.0% (95% CI, 85.1 to 99.3)]. These findings have clinical implications for early risk assessment and the feasibility of developing early preventative interventions for ASD.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 2014

Repetitive Behavior in 12-Month-Olds Later Classified With Autism Spectrum Disorder

Jed T. Elison; Jason J. Wolff; J. Steven Reznick; Kelly N. Botteron; Annette Estes; Hongbin Gu; Heather Cody Hazlett; Adriane J. Meadows; Sarah J. Paterson; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; Joseph Piven

OBJECTIVE As compared to the utility of early emerging social communicative risk markers for predicting a later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), less is known about the relevance of early patterns of restricted and repetitive behaviors. We examined patterns of stereotyped motor mannerisms and repetitive manipulation of objects in 12-month-olds at high and low risk for developing ASD, all of whom were assessed for ASD at 24 months. METHOD Observational coding of repetitive object manipulation and stereotyped motor behaviors in digital recordings of the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales was conducted using the Repetitive and Stereotyped Movement Scales for 3 groups of 12-month-olds: low-risk infants (LR, n = 53); high-familial-risk infants who did not meet diagnostic criteria for ASD at 24 months (HR-negative, n = 75); and high-familial-risk infants who met diagnostic criteria for ASD at 24 months (HR-ASD, n = 30). RESULTS The HR-ASD group showed significantly more stereotyped motor mannerisms than both the HR-negative group (p = .025) and the LR group (p = .001). The HR-ASD and HR-negative groups demonstrated statistically equivalent repetitive object manipulation scores (p = .431), and both groups showed significantly more repetitive object manipulation than the LR group (p < .040). Combining the motor and object stereotypy scores into a Repetitive and Stereotyped Movement Scales (RSMS) composite yielded a disorder-continuum effect such that each group was significantly different from one another (LR < HR-negative < HR-ASD). CONCLUSION These results suggest that targeted assessment of repetitive behavior during infancy may augment early ASD identification efforts.


Biological Psychiatry | 2017

Increased Extra-axial Cerebrospinal Fluid in High-Risk Infants Who Later Develop Autism

Mark D. Shen; Sun Hyung Kim; Robert C. McKinstry; Hongbin Gu; Heather C. Hazlett; Christine W. Nordahl; Robert W. Emerson; Dennis Shaw; Jed T. Elison; Meghan R. Swanson; Vladimir S. Fonov; Guido Gerig; Stephen R. Dager; Kelly N. Botteron; Sarah Paterson; Robert T. Schultz; Alan C. Evans; Annette M. Estes; Lonnie Zwaigenbaum; Martin A. Styner; David G. Amaral; Joseph Piven; Heather Cody Hazlett; C. Chappell; Annette Estes; D. A. Shaw; Kelly Botteron; R. McKinstry; J. Constantino; J. Pruett

BACKGROUND We previously reported that infants who developed autism spectrum disorder (ASD) had increased cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) in the subarachnoid space (i.e., extra-axial CSF) from 6 to 24 months of age. We attempted to confirm and extend this finding in a larger independent sample. METHODS A longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of infants at risk for ASD was carried out on 343 infants, who underwent neuroimaging at 6, 12, and 24 months. Of these infants, 221 were at high risk for ASD because of an older sibling with ASD, and 122 were at low risk with no family history of ASD. A total of 47 infants were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months and were compared with 174 high-risk and 122 low-risk infants without ASD. RESULTS Infants who developed ASD had significantly greater extra-axial CSF volume at 6 months compared with both comparison groups without ASD (18% greater than high-risk infants without ASD; Cohens d = 0.54). Extra-axial CSF volume remained elevated through 24 months (d = 0.46). Infants with more severe autism symptoms had an even greater volume of extra-axial CSF from 6 to 24 months (24% greater at 6 months, d = 0.70; 15% greater at 24 months, d = 0.70). Extra-axial CSF volume at 6 months predicted which high-risk infants would be diagnosed with ASD at 24 months with an overall accuracy of 69% and corresponding 66% sensitivity and 68% specificity, which was fully cross-validated in a separate sample. CONCLUSIONS This study confirms and extends previous findings that increased extra-axial CSF is detectable at 6 months in high-risk infants who develop ASD. Future studies will address whether this anomaly is a contributing factor to the etiology of ASD or an early risk marker for ASD.


Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience | 2015

Accurate age classification of 6 and 12 month-old infants based on resting-state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging data

John R. Pruett; Sridhar Kandala; Sarah Hoertel; Abraham Z. Snyder; Jed T. Elison; Tomoyuki Nishino; Eric Feczko; Nico U.F. Dosenbach; Binyam Nardos; Jonathan D. Power; Babatunde Adeyemo; Kelly N. Botteron; Robert C. McKinstry; Alan C. Evans; Heather Cody Hazlett; Stephen R. Dager; Sarah Paterson; Robert T. Schultz; D. Louis Collins; Vladimir Fonov; Martin Styner; Guido Gerig; Samir Das; Penelope Kostopoulos; John N. Constantino; Annette Estes; Steven E. Petersen; Bradley L. Schlaggar; Joseph Piven

Highlights • SVMs classified 6 versus 12 month-old infants above chance based on fcMRI data alone.• We carefully accounted for the effects of fcMRI motion artifact.• These results coincide with a period of dramatic change in infant development.• Two interpretations about connections supporting this age categorization are given.

Collaboration


Dive into the Jed T. Elison's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Joseph Piven

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Annette Estes

University of Washington

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Heather Cody Hazlett

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robert T. Schultz

Children's Hospital of Philadelphia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kelly N. Botteron

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sarah Paterson

University of Pennsylvania

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Martin Styner

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge