Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl.


Journal of Personality | 2011

Constructing Stories of Self-Growth: How Individual Differences in Patterns of Autobiographical Reasoning Relate to Well-being in Midlife

Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Dan P. McAdams

Although growth has been a central focus in narrative research, few studies have examined growth comprehensively, as a story that emerges across the interpretation of many events. In this study, we examined how individual differences in autobiographical reasoning (AR) about self-growth relate to traits and well-being in a national sample of midlife adults (N= 88) who ranged in age from 34 to 68. Two patterns of growth-related AR were identified: (1) positive processing, defined as the average tendency to interpret events positively (vs. negatively), and (2) differentiated processing, defined as the extent to which past events are interpreted as causing a variety of forms of self-growth. Results showed that positive processing was negatively related to neuroticism and predicted well-being even after controlling for the average valence of past events. Additionally, differentiated processing of negative events but not positive events was positively related to openness and predictive of well-being. Finally, growth-related AR patterns independently predicted well-being beyond the effects of traits and demographic factors.


Memory | 2008

Why recall our highs and lows: Relations between memory functions, age, and well-being

Kate C. McLean; Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl

This study examined whether positive and negative memories (life story high and low points) were differentially used for reminiscence functions concerning self and social aspects of reminiscing, and relations between function use and well-being in two age groups. Life story high and low points were collected from a sample of emerging (n =56) and older (n =55) adults, as well as a measure of the use of these memories for the self-functions of death preparation, identity, and problem solving, and the social functions of conversation and teach/inform, and a measure of psychological well-being. Memories were also coded for whether or not they contained a redemptive narrative structure (from emotionally negative to emotionally positive). Results showed that the endorsement of reminiscence functions did differ by memory type, with high points more often endorsed for the functions of identity, teach/inform, and conversation than low points. These main effects were qualified by memory type × age interactions. The use of these functions for each kind of memory was also related to well-being, but differentially for older and younger people, and redemptive sequencing was especially important to the well-being of the younger group. Findings are discussed in terms of the importance of different emotional memories for self and well-being at different points in the lifespan.


Memory | 2013

When is meaning making unhealthy for the self? The roles of neuroticism, implicit theories, and memory telling in trauma and transgression memories

Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Kate C. McLean; Cade D. Mansfield

The aim of this study was to examine how three factors—neuroticism, implicit theories of personality, and memory telling—relate to patterns of healthy and unhealthy meaning making in two kinds of negative memories: traumas and transgressions. Healthy meaning making was defined as self-growth, whereas unhealthy meaning making was defined as viewing the self as damaged in traumas (damaged self) and as a bad person in transgressions (bad self). A total of 85 adult participants completed survey measures of personality and memory telling (retrospective reports of extent to which memory was shared with others) and wrote a narrative of a trauma and a transgression which were coded for specific forms of meaning making. Results revealed distinct patterns of associations for trauma and transgression memories. The combination of low neuroticism and an incremental theory (belief that personality can change) predicted self-growth in traumas, whereas memory telling was predictive of self-growth in transgressions, especially among incremental theorists. For unhealthy forms of meaning making, an entity theory (belief that personality is fixed) was associated with the bad self in transgressions, and viewing the self as damaged by traumas was more common among younger adults than older adults.


Journal of Personality | 2013

Does ego development increase during midlife? The effects of openness and accommodative processing of difficult events.

Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Ravenna Helson; Oliver P. John

OBJECTIVE Although Loevingers model of ego development is a theory of personality growth, there are few studies that have examined age-related change in ego level over developmentally significant periods of adulthood. To address this gap in the literature, we examined mean-level change and individual differences in change in ego level over 18 years of midlife. METHOD In this longitudinal study, participants were 79 predominantly White, college-educated women who completed the Washington University Sentence Completion Test in early (age 43) and late (age 61) midlife as well as measures of the trait of Openness (ages 21, 43, 52, and 61) and accommodative processing (assessed from narratives of difficult life events at age 52). RESULTS As hypothesized, the sample overall showed a mean-level increase in ego level from age 43 to age 61. Additionally, a regression analysis showed that both the trait of Openness at age 21 and accommodative processing of difficult events that occurred during (as opposed to prior to) midlife were each predictive of increasing ego level from age 43 to age 61. CONCLUSIONS These findings counter prior claims that ego level remains stable during adulthood and contribute to our understanding of the underlying processes involved in personality growth in midlife.


Social Psychological and Personality Science | 2017

Research Methods for Studying Narrative Identity: A Primer

Jonathan M. Adler; William L. Dunlop; Robyn Fivush; Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Jennifer Lodi-Smith; Dan P. McAdams; Kate C. McLean; Monisha Pasupathi; Moin Syed

This article provides a primer for researchers seeking an introduction to quantitative narrative research methods. It represents a consensus document of most common practices used by the coauthors. Key elements of conducting narrative research (e.g., asking narrative questions, designing narrative prompts, collecting narratives, coding narratives) are discussed along with limitations to this approach and future directions.


Journal of Personality | 2018

Identity Development in Cultural Context: The Role of Deviating from Master Narratives

Kate C. McLean; Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Chelsea Fordham; Elizabeth Alpert; Emma Marsden; Kathryn Szymanowski; Dan P. McAdams

OBJECTIVE The great majority of research on identity and personality development has focused on individual processes of development, to the relative neglect of the cultural context of development. We employ a recently articulated framework for the examination of identity development in context, centered on the construct of master narratives, or culturally shared stories. METHOD Across four studies, we asked emerging and midlife adults (N = 512) to narrate personal experiences of deviations from these master narratives. RESULTS Across three quantitative studies, we show that (a) those who elaborated their deviation experiences were more likely to be in structurally marginalized positions in society (e.g., ethnic or sexual minorities); (b) those who elaborated an empowering alternative to the master narrative were more likely to be engaged in identity processes; and (c) master narratives maintain their rigidity by the frequency of their use. In study 4, using qualitative analyses, we illustrate the rigidity of master narratives, as well as the degree to which they take shape in social and group experiences. CONCLUSIONS These studies emphasize the importance of cultural context in considering personality and identity development.


Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2018

“So Now, I Wonder, What Am I?”: A Narrative Approach to Bicultural Identity Integration

Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Verónica Benet-Martínez; Margaret Bishop; Katharine Gilson; Lindsay Festa; Chloe Levenson; Rena Rosenblum

Individuals socialized in multiple cultures actively construct their bicultural identity in the context of relevant life events. However, the content and meaning of these experiences, as subjectively constructed and understood by the individual, remain largely unexplored in relation to biculturalism outcomes. Using a narrative approach, two studies of U.S. biculturals examined how memories about the experience of being bicultural (“bicultural memory narratives”) related to bicultural identity integration (BII). BII describes having cultural identities that are blended and exist harmoniously within the self (vs. having dissociated and conflicting identities). In Study 1, bicultural college students shared two bicultural memory narratives, which were coded for the past event valence, exploratory processing, and ending valence (positive vs. negative ending). Most reported memories about at least somewhat negative experiences, and yet narrating those events with positive endings was predictive of higher BII, even after controlling for other factors, such as trait affect. In Study 2, second-generation bicultural adults ranging in age from 18 to 62 shared narratives about memories of bicultural conflict, which were also coded for exploratory processing and positive resolution. Similar to Study 1, positive resolution predicted higher levels of BII, independent of other factors. Bicultural identity conflict was found to decrease with age across adulthood. Findings for exploratory processing were inconclusive and suggest that its role in BII may fluctuate over time and be dependent on several factors, including age, type of memory, and time spent exploring bicultural identity.


Psychological Inquiry | 2004

The transformed self: A narrative understanding of posttraumatic growth

Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Dan P. McAdams


Narrative Inquiry | 2010

Narrating traumas and transgressions: Links between narrative processing, wisdom, and well-being

Cade D. Mansfield; Kate C. McLean; Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl


Archive | 1998

How are dimensions of adult personality related to ego development? An application of the typological approach

Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl; Oliver P. John

Collaboration


Dive into the Jennifer Pals Lilgendahl's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kate C. McLean

Western Washington University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Cade D. Mansfield

Western Washington University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Oliver P. John

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Chelsea Fordham

Western Washington University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jonathan M. Adler

Franklin W. Olin College of Engineering

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge