Jerry M. Crutchfield
Federal Aviation Administration
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Jerry M. Crutchfield.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2003
M. Kathryn Bleckley; Francis T. Durso; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Randall W. Engle; Maya M. Khanna
To the extent that individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) reflect differences in attention (Baddeley, 1993; Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999), differences in WMC should predict performance on visual attention tasks. Individuals who scored in the upper and lower quartiles on the OSPAN working memory test performed a modification of Egly and Homa’s (1984) selective attention task. In this task, the participants identified a central letter and localized a displaced letter flashed somewhere on one of three concentric rings. When the displaced letter occurred closer to fixation than the cue implied, high-WMC, but not low-WMC, individuals showed a cost in the letter localization task. This suggests that low-WMC participants allocated attention as a spotlight, whereas those with high WMC showed flexible allocation.
Human Factors | 2008
Francis T. Durso; Arathi Sethumadhavan; Jerry M. Crutchfield
Objective: The main objective of this study is to present a methodology for computing information relevance. Background: Relevance is a pervasive term used in several domains, such as pragmatics, information science, and psychology. Quantifying the relevance of information can be helpful in effective display design. Displays should be designed so that the more relevant information is more easily accessed. Method: This procedure focuses on computing the relevance of a piece of information by taking into account three aspects of tasks that use the information: the number of different tasks that make use of the information, the frequency of occurrence of those tasks, and the criticality of those tasks. The methodology can be used to compute the aggregate relevance of a piece of information for a particular component of a system or for the entire system. This methodology was illustrated using the domain of air traffic control (ATC). Results: In support of the validity of the methodology, we were able to confirm the value of weather information and traffic information in ATC towers. Conclusion: The method can be used to derive information relevance, a characteristic of information that has implications for display design for any domain. Application: Designers can use information about aggregate relevance to design information displays that feature the most relevant information.
Human Factors | 2004
Francis T. Durso; Peter J. Batsakes; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Justin Braden; Carol A. Manning
The Federal Aviation Administrations effort to automate air traffic control (ATC) requires that the functionality provided today be captured in future systems. We report the first quantitative naturalistic observation of paper flight progress strip interactions during operational use. Strip use was similar in a variety of situations, but some uses varied as a function of altitude, staffing, or the cooperative style used by controller teams. Design of automation should proceed by prioritizing changes based on frequency of use and importance and should ensure that an effective method of interacting with flight information is incorporated. In addition to applied relevance to the ATC domain, the results touch on several theoretical concerns relevant to dynamic environments. Actual and potential applications of this research include the establishment of a database of strip activity and an arsenal of information valuable to system designers.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied | 2010
Francis T. Durso; Brian R. Johnson; Jerry M. Crutchfield
In an effort to determine the information needs of tower air traffic controllers, instructors from the Federal Aviation Administrations Academy in Oklahoma City were asked to control traffic in a high-fidelity tower cab simulator. Information requests were made apparent by eliminating access to standard tower information sources. Instead, controllers were required to ask for precisely the information they needed during the scenarios. The information requests were classified using an elaboration of Zwaan and Radvanskys (1998) dimensions of situation models. The vast majority of requests were about three of the dimensions originally developed for reading comprehension: the protagonist, intentionality, and space. The information requests were also classified into 28 operational categories (e.g., aircraft identification, destination). From these results, the data were summarized, not just statistically, but by the creation of display-hypotheses. The display-hypotheses were organized according to the situation-model dimensions. Controllers preferred data blocks organized by the situation-model principle over those that violated this organization. The summary display-hypotheses were quite simple and accounted for the vast majority of the information requests controllers made. The display-hypotheses accounted for the information needs of controllers during routine as well as off-nominal events.
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting | 2001
Francis T. Durso; Peter J. Batsakes; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Justin Braden; Carol A. Manning
Subject matter experts were employed to record observations of flight progress strip marking across 5 en route ATC facilities. Approximately 220 hours of ATC observations were recorded establishing a group of high frequency/high importance markings. These markings were perceived by controllers to provide performance benefits through externalizing memory and communication. The results are discussed in relation to possible electronic alternatives to flight progress strips.
Air traffic control quarterly | 1998
Francis T. Durso; Carla A. Hackworth; Todd R. Truitt; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Danko Nikolić
The International Journal of Aviation Psychology | 1998
Francis T. Durso; Todd R. Truitt; Carla A. Hackworth; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Christine A. Manning
Archive | 1995
Francis T. Durso; Todd R. Truitt; Carla A. Hackworth; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Daryl D. Ohrt; Danko Nikolić; Peter M. Moertl; Carol A. Manning
Air traffic control quarterly | 2000
Todd R. Truitt; Francis T. Durso; Jerry M. Crutchfield; Peter M. Moertl; Carol A. Manning
Air traffic control quarterly | 2011
Francis T. Durso; Arathi Sethumadhavan; Jerry M. Crutchfield; John Morris