Jill A. Warker
University of Scranton
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Jill A. Warker.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2006
Jill A. Warker; Gary S. Dell
Speech errors reveal the speakers implicit knowledge of phonotactic constraints, both languagewide constraints (e.g., /K/ cannot be a syllable onset when one is speaking English) and experimentally induced constraints (e.g., /k/ cannot be an onset during the experiment). Four experiments investigated the acquisition of novel 2nd-order constraints, in which the allowable position of a consonant depends on some other property of the syllable (e.g., /k/ can only be an onset if the vowel is /I/). Participants recited strings of syllables that exhibited the novel constraints throughout a 4-day experiment. Their errors reflected the newly learned constraints but not until the 2nd day of training. This contrasts with previous research showing that errors become sensitive to 1st-order constraints almost immediately. A model that learns to assign phonemes to syllable positions is presented. It attributes the relative slowness of the acquisition of 2nd-order constraints to the self-interfering property of these constraints.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2008
Jill A. Warker; Gary S. Dell; Christine A. Whalen; Samantha Gereg
Adults can learn new artificial phonotactic constraints by producing syllables that exhibit the constraints. The experiments presented here tested the limits of phonotactic learning in production using speech errors as an implicit measure of learning. Experiment 1 tested a constraint in which the placement of a consonant as an onset or coda depended on the identity of a nonadjacent consonant. Participant speech errors reflected knowledge of the constraint but not until the 2nd day of testing. Experiment 2 tested a constraint in which consonant placement depended on an extralinguistic factor, the speech rate. Participants were not able to learn this constraint. Together, these experiments suggest that phonotactic-like constraints are acquired when mutually constraining elements reside within the phonological system.
Psychological Science | 2014
M. Gareth Gaskell; Jill A. Warker; Shane Lindsay; Rebecca Frost; James Guest; Reza Snowdon; Abigail Stackhouse
The constraints that govern acceptable phoneme combinations in speech perception and production have considerable plasticity. We addressed whether sleep influences the acquisition of new constraints and their integration into the speech-production system. Participants repeated sequences of syllables in which two phonemes were artificially restricted to syllable onset or syllable coda, depending on the vowel in that sequence. After 48 sequences, participants either had a 90-min nap or remained awake. Participants then repeated 96 sequences so implicit constraint learning could be examined, and then were tested for constraint generalization in a forced-choice task. The sleep group, but not the wake group, produced speech errors at test that were consistent with restrictions on the placement of phonemes in training. Furthermore, only the sleep group generalized their learning to new materials. Polysomnography data showed that implicit constraint learning was associated with slow-wave sleep. These results show that sleep facilitates the integration of new linguistic knowledge with existing production constraints. These data have relevance for systems-consolidation models of sleep.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2013
Jill A. Warker
Adults can rapidly learn artificial phonotactic constraints such as /f/ occurs only at the beginning of syllables by producing syllables that contain those constraints. This implicit learning is then reflected in their speech errors. However, second-order constraints in which the placement of a phoneme depends on another characteristic of the syllable (e.g., if the vowel is /æ/, /f/ occurs at the beginning of syllables and /s/ occurs at the end of syllables, but if the vowel is /I/, the reverse is true) require a longer learning period. Two experiments investigated the transience of second-order learning and whether consolidation plays a role in learning phonological dependencies, with speech errors used as a measure of learning. Experiment 1 tested the durability of learning and found that learning was still present in speech errors a week later. Experiment 2 looked at whether more time in the form of a consolidation period or more experience in the form of more trials was necessary for learning to be revealed in speech errors. Both consolidation and more trials led to learning; however, consolidation provided a more substantial benefit.
Memory & Cognition | 2013
Laura Mickes; Ryan S. Darby; Vivian Hwe; Daniel Bajic; Jill A. Warker; Christine R. Harris; Nicholas Christenfeld
Online social networking is vastly popular and permits its members to post their thoughts as microblogs, an opportunity that people exploit, on Facebook alone, over 30 million times an hour. Such trivial ephemera, one might think, should vanish quickly from memory; conversely, they may comprise the sort of information that our memories are tuned to recognize, if that which we readily generate, we also readily store. In the first two experiments, participants’ memory for Facebook posts was found to be strikingly stronger than their memory for human faces or sentences from books—a magnitude comparable to the difference in memory strength between amnesics and healthy controls. The second experiment suggested that this difference is not due to Facebook posts spontaneously generating social elaboration, because memory for posts is enhanced as much by adding social elaboration as is memory for book sentences. Our final experiment, using headlines, sentences, and reader comments from articles, suggested that the remarkable memory for microblogs is also not due to their completeness or simply their topic, but may be a more general phenomenon of their being the largely spontaneous and natural emanations of the human mind.
Memory & Cognition | 2017
Iva Ivanova; Liane Wardlow; Jill A. Warker; Victor S. Ferreira
Speakers sometimes encounter utterances that have anomalous linguistic features. Are such features registered during comprehension and transferred to speakers’ production systems? In two experiments, we explored these questions. In a syntactic-priming paradigm, speakers heard prime sentences with novel or intransitive verbs as part of prepositional-dative or double-object structures (e.g., The chef munded the cup to the burglar or The doctor existed the pirate the balloon). Speakers then described target pictures eliciting the same structures, using the same or different novel or intransitive verbs. Speakers overall described targets with the same structures as the primes (abstract syntactic priming), but more so when the primes and targets had the same novel or intransitive verbs (a lexical boost), an effect that was only observed when the novel words served as the verbs in both the prime and target sentences. Such a lexical boost could only manifest if speakers formed associations between the verbs and structures in the primes during comprehension, and if these associations were then transferred to their production systems. We thus showed that anomalous utterance features are not ignored but persist (at least) in speakers’ immediately subsequent production.
Journal of Sleep Research | 2012
Rebecca Frost; Gareth Gaskell; Jill A. Warker; James Guest; Reza Snowdon; Abbi Stackhouse
Objective: This study investigated group delivery of a mindfulnessbased intervention for primary insomnia in an Australian population. Mindfulness Based Therapy for Insomnia (MBT-I) offers an alternative approach to the current gold standard, non-pharmacological approach to insomnia, CBT-I, with a focus on reducing sleep-related arousal. Findings have indicated reductions in several subjective sleep measures following MBT-I treatment (Ong, Shapiro, & Manber, 2008). This study aimed to investigate the outcome of MBT-I in an Australian population recruited from a sleep clinic to examine the generalizability in a diverse sample. Participants: 30 participants, consisting of 21 females (M age = 50, range = 26–72) and nine males (M age = 45, range 34–59) who met criteria for primary insomnia. Methods: Treatment consisted of six sessions of MBT-I (Ong, Shapiro, & Manber, 2008) delivered in groups of 7–8 with each session lasting 2 h in duration. The primary outcome measure was the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) and secondary the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Outcome measures were recorded at four timepoints (screening, baseline, post-treatment and 3 month follow-up). Results: The average severity of insomnia as measured by the ISI reduced significantly from a moderate level of insomnia (M = 18.74) to sub-clinical insomnia (M = 12.79, P < 0.01) indicating that on average, participants no longer met the criteria for insomnia following treatment. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) overall score reduced significantly (M = 13.1 to M = 9.2, P < 0.01) reflecting an increase in sleep quality following treatment. All 7 PSQI component scores reduced significantly. The largest change was the component score assessing sleep efficiency (the proportion of average sleep compared to time in bed), which increased from 72% to 83% (P < 0.01) following treatment. Conclusion: Analysis of data collected in response to a group treatment of MBT-I for insomnia delivered over 6 weeks revealed significant reductions in insomnia symptoms, and improvements in sleep quality and sleep efficiency. This suggests that MBT-I can be delivered in a sleep clinic setting with indications of effectiveness.
Cognition | 2009
Jill A. Warker; Ye Xu; Gary S. Dell; Cynthia Fisher
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2015
Jill A. Warker; Gary S. Dell
Lot Occasional Series | 2004
Gary S. Dell; Jill A. Warker