Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Joel Erblich is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Joel Erblich.


Annals of Behavioral Medicine | 2000

Looking forward and back: Distress among women at familial risk for breast cancer

Joel Erblich; Dana H. Bovbjerg; Heiddis B. Valdimarsdottir

Healthy women with family histories of breast cancer in a first-degree relative (FH+) have been reported to exhibit higher levels of breast cancer-related distress than women without family histories of breast cancer (FH−). Recent data suggest that this may be particularly true for women who had parent die of cancer. In live with theories emphasizing the psychological impacts of past stressors and concerns for the future, the present study examined the hypotheses that past cancer stressors (i.e. maternal breast cancer caregiving and death, “Looking Back”) and perceptions of one’s own heightened future risk for developing the disease (“Looking Forward”) would predict current levels of distress. One hundred forty-eight healthy women (57 FH+, 91 FH−) recruited from large medical centers in the New York City area completed measures of breast cancer-related distress, general psychological distress, and items assessing whether or not they had taken care of their mother with breast cancer or had had their mother die from the disease. Consistent with previous research, results indicated that FH+ women whose mothers had died of breast cancer had significantly higher breast cancer-related distress than either FH+ women whose mothers had not died of breast cancer or FH−women (p<.05). Further analyses revealed that FH+ women who had cared for their mothers with breast cancer had higher cancer-related distress than women who did not (p<.01), and that FH+ women whose experience included both caregiving and the death of their mother from breast cancer had the highest levels of cancer-related distress (p<.01) and depressive symptoms (p<.05). Findings also indicated that FH+ women with heightened perceptions of risk for breast cancer had higher levels of distress, independent of past stressors. These findings suggest that psychosocial interventions for women with family histories of breast cancer might be appropriately focused on these issues.


Preventive Medicine | 2003

Family and friends with disease:: their impact on perceived risk

Guy H. Montgomery; Joel Erblich; Terry A. DiLorenzo; Dana H. Bovbjerg

BACKGROUND For many common diseases, having a family history is the strongest predictor of lifetime risk. Perceptions of personal risk, important for appropriate prevention efforts, have been found to be exaggerated in healthy individuals with family histories. These findings highlight the contribution of objective and experiential factors to perceived risk. This study examined, across a variety of diseases, whether (1) family history of the disease contributes to perceived risk, (2) history of disease in a friend or nonblood relative, which would not increase ones objective risk, nonetheless increases perceived risk, and (3) these effects are similar across genders. METHODS Participants (N = 522; 38% male; 56% Caucasian; mean age = 40 years) completed a brief health survey. RESULTS Analyses revealed an effect of having a family history of the disease on perceived risk for breast and colon cancers, heart disease, and diabetes (P < 0.001). Interestingly, having a friend diagnosed with the disease also contributed to perceived risk for breast and colon cancers, as well as heart disease and diabetes among women (P < 0.05), but not among men. CONCLUSIONS Results suggest that interventions to alter perceived risk of cancer should account for gender, as women appear to be impacted by who they know.


Journal of Behavioral Medicine | 2000

Psychological Distress, Health Beliefs, and Frequency of Breast Self-Examination

Joel Erblich; Dana H. Bovbjerg; Heiddis B. Valdimarsdottir

Although monthly breast self-examination (BSE) is recommended for early breast cancer detection, most women do not comply. Few studies have examined the impact of psychological distress on BSE frequency. Recent research suggests that it may be particularly important to examine the role of distress in the recently identified phenomenon of BSE overperformance (>1/month). One hundred thirty-five healthy women with and without family histories of breast cancer completed sociodemographic, health belief, general and cancer-specific psychological distress, and BSE frequency questionnaires. The central finding of the study was that BSE underperformance and overperformance had two distinct sets of predictors: health beliefs, specifically barriers against BSE and low confidence in BSE performance, were related to BSE underperformance, while higher levels of psychological distress, particularly cancer-specific intrusive thoughts, were related to BSE overperformance. Findings underscore the need to evaluate BSE under- and overperformance separately and to develop problem-specific interventions to increase compliance with monthly BSE.


Molecular Psychiatry | 2005

Effects of dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) and transporter (SLC6A3) polymorphisms on smoking cue-induced cigarette craving among African-American smokers.

Joel Erblich; Caryn Lerman; David W. Self; George A. Diaz; Dana H. Bovbjerg

Cue-induced craving for addictive substances has long been known to contribute to the problem of persistent addiction in humans. Research in animals over the past decade has solidly established the central role of dopamine in cue-induced craving for addictive substances, including nicotine. Analogous studies in humans, however, are lacking, especially among African-American smokers, who have lower quit rates than Caucasian smokers. Based on the animal literature, the studys objective was to test the hypothesis that smokers carrying specific variants in dopamine-related genes previously associated with risk for addictive behaviors would exhibit heightened levels of cigarette craving following laboratory exposure to cues. To this end, cigarette craving was induced in healthy African-American smokers (n=88) through laboratory exposure to smoking cues. Smokers carrying either the DRD2 (D2 dopamine receptor gene) TaqI A1 RFLP or the SLC6A3 (dopamine transporter gene) 9-repeat VNTR polymorphisms had stronger cue-induced cravings than noncarriers (Ps <0.05 and 0.01, respectively). Consistent with the separate biological pathways involved (receptor, transporter), carriers of both polymorphisms had markedly higher craving responses compared to those with neither (P<0.0006), reflecting additive effects. Findings provide support for the role of dopamine in cue-induced craving in humans, and suggest a possible genetic risk factor for persistent smoking behavior in African-American smokers.


Pharmacogenomics Journal | 2004

Stress-induced cigarette craving: effects of the DRD2 TaqI RFLP and SLC6A3 VNTR polymorphisms

Joel Erblich; Caryn Lerman; David W. Self; George A. Diaz; Dana H. Bovbjerg

ABSTRACTAnimal models have long implicated dopamine in stress-induced craving for a variety of addictive substances. However, translational studies of dopamine, stress and craving in humans are lacking. Based on the animal literature, this studys objective was to test the hypothesis that cigarette smokers carrying specific variants in dopamine-related genes would have heightened levels of cigarette craving following exposure to a laboratory stressor. Cigarette craving induced by controlled exposure to a laboratory stressor was assessed in healthy adult smokers (n=108) recruited by advertisement. Significantly stronger stress-induced cigarette craving was found for individuals carrying either the DRD2 (D2 dopamine receptor gene) A1, or the SLC6A3 (dopamine transporter gene) nine-repeat allelic variants. Stress-induced craving was markedly higher for those carrying both alleles, compared to those with neither, consistent with the separate biological pathways involved (receptor, transporter). Findings provide strong support for the possibility that dopamine involvement in stress-induced craving well established in animal models also applies to humans, and suggest a potential genetic risk factor for persistent smoking behavior.


Journal of Behavioral Medicine | 2006

A Model of Disease-Specific Worry in Heritable Disease: The Influence of Family History, Perceived Risk and Worry About Other Illnesses

Terry A. DiLorenzo; Julie B. Schnur; Guy H. Montgomery; Joel Erblich; Gary Winkel; Dana H. Bovbjerg

Disease-related worry is associated with family history and perceived risk of that disease; however, the influences of general risk perceptions and tendencies to worry about diseases have been neglected in the literature. This study investigates a model of disease-specific worry which includes family history, disease-specific perceived risk, and perceived risk for and worry about other diseases. Participants completed a survey assessing these variables in relation to several heritable diseases. Structural equation modeling found that family history predicted disease-specific perceived risk but not perceived risk for other diseases. Disease-specific perceived risk predicted disease-specific worry and worry about other diseases. Perceived risk for other diseases predicted worry about other diseases and disease-specific perceived risk but not disease-specific worry. Disease-specific worry predicted worry about other diseases. This model was supported across several diseases and indicates that disease-specific and general considerations of risk influence worry about a disease and should be considered in interventions.


Health Psychology | 2003

Biased cognitive processing of cancer-related information among women with family histories of breast cancer: Evidence from a cancer Stroop task.

Joel Erblich; Guy H. Montgomery; Heiddis B. Valdimarsdottir; Marylene Cloitre; Dana H. Bovbjerg

Stimuli associated with sources of stress have been shown to interfere with cognition. The authors hypothesized that women with the stress of having a family history of breast cancer (FH+) would exhibit greater interference on a task with cancer-related stimuli than women without cancer in the family (FH-). The authors developed a modified Stroop color-naming task to test this hypothesis in a sample of FH+ (n = 72) and FH- (n = 96) women. Consistent with the hypotheses, FH+ women had longer color-naming times and more errors (ps < .01) on a cancer word list relative to noncancer lists. This biased processing was not mediated by the significantly higher perceived risk, general distress, or cancer-specific distress in FH+ women. Maladaptive alterations in processing cancer stimuli may have important clinical implications, as these women must process complex cancer-related information critical to their health (e.g., options for chemoprevention, screening).


Addictive Behaviors | 2003

Biphasic stimulant and sedative effects of ethanol: Are children of alcoholics really different?

Joel Erblich; Mitchell Earleywine; Bryna Erblich; Dana H. Bovbjerg

Children of alcoholics (COAs) have an increased risk of developing alcoholism themselves. The mechanisms responsible are not yet known. One compelling theory postulates that COAs may have an increased sensitivity to the stimulant effects of alcohol during the ascending limb of the blood alcohol curve combined with a decreased sensitivity to the putatively undesirable sedative effects of the drug during the descending limb, providing a particularly strong motivation to drink. Consistent with this theory, we hypothesized that compared to children of nonalcoholics (CONAs), COAs would display higher levels of ascending limb stimulation and lower levels of descending limb sedation. In the present study, 100 college students, who were either COAs (n=18) or CONAs (n=82), completed the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (a self-report measure of stimulation and sedation): (1) before consuming 0.85n ml/kg ethanol; (2) during the ascending limb of their BAC, and; (3) during the descending limb of their BAC. Although findings indicated that COAs and CONAs had comparable levels of sedation at each time point, a significant GroupxTime interaction (P<.02) indicated that COAs had greater increases in stimulation from baseline than CONAs, providing partial support for our hypothesis. Interestingly, simple effects analyses revealed that COAs had lower baseline levels of stimulation but almost identical levels of ascending and descending limb stimulation as CONAs, suggesting that increased sensitivity to alcohol among COAs may be the result of baseline understimulation. Overall, findings suggest that theorized differences between COAs and CONAs may be due in part to broader trait differences or other nonpharmacological factors.


Biological Psychology | 2007

Poor sleep the night before an experimental stress task is associated with reduced cortisol reactivity in healthy women.

Caroline E. Wright; Heiddis B. Valdimarsdottir; Joel Erblich; Dana H. Bovbjerg

Sleep disruption is a growing problem that may have serious health effects. As stress-induced increases in cortisol are thought to be a key adaptive process it is important to examine how this response is affected by sleep. The current study investigated the association of four sleep parameters (objective/subjectively measured sleep quality and quantity) and subsequent salivary cortisol reactivity (maximal change from baseline) to an experimental stressor in 53 healthy women. Objective actigraphy monitoring and self-report diaries were used to assess sleep. Results revealed that individuals with lower objective sleep quality (wake percentage during sleep) had a blunted response to the experimental stressor. No associations were found between cortisol reactivity and actigraphy-derived sleep quantity, or either of the self-reported sleep variables. Results are discussed with regard to the possible adverse health effects that may result from poor sleep quality and a blunted cortisol response to stress.


Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology | 2004

In vivo versus imaginal smoking cue exposures: is seeing believing?

Joel Erblich; Dana H. Bovbjerg

Smokers experience cigarette cravings in response to both imaginal and in vivo cigarette cues, and some studies suggest that the magnitude of this reactivity relates to difficulty quitting. Few studies, however, have systematically examined these two paradigms head-to-head. To this end, the authors exposed 225 smokers to imaginal and in vivo smoking cues and measured craving reactions. Results indicated that both imaginal and in vivo smoking cues increased craving. The magnitude of imaginal and in vivo reactions were statistically comparable and were moderately correlated. In vivo, but not imaginal, reactivity was related to duration of previous quits, particularly in men. Findings suggest that although both paradigms induce craving, the in vivo reactivity paradigm may be, at least in men, more effective for predicting smoking cessation failure.

Collaboration


Dive into the Joel Erblich's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dana H. Bovbjerg

Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Guy H. Montgomery

Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mitchell Earleywine

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel David

Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Devorah E. Yarmush

City University of New York

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

George A. Diaz

Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Julie B. Schnur

Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge