Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Johannes Lyzenga is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Johannes Lyzenga.


Hearing Research | 2005

Effect of inter-phase gap on the sensitivity of cochlear implant users to electrical stimulation

Robert P. Carlyon; Astrid Van Wieringen; John M. Deeks; Christopher J. Long; Johannes Lyzenga; Jan Wouters

Human behavioral thresholds for trains of biphasic pulses applied to a single channel of Nucleus CI24 and LAURA cochlear implants were measured as a function of inter-phase gap (IPG). Experiment 1 used bipolar stimulation, a 100-pps pulse rate, and a 400-ms stimulus duration. In one condition, the two phases of each pulse had opposite polarity. Thresholds continued to drop by 9-10 dB as IPG was increased from near zero to the longest value tested (2900 micros for CI24, 4900 micros for LAURA). This time course is much longer than reported for single-cell recordings from animals. In a second condition, the two phases of each pulse had the same polarity, which alternated from pulse to pulse. Thresholds were independent of IPG, and similar to those in condition 1 at IPG=4900 micros. Experiment 2 used monopolar stimulation. One condition was similar to condition 1 of experiment 1, and thresholds also dropped up to the longest IPG studied (2900 micros). This also happened when the pulse rate was reduced to 20 pps, and when only a single pulse was presented on each trial. Keeping IPG constant at 8 micros and adding an extra biphasic pulse x ms into each period produced thresholds that were roughly independent of x, indicating that the effect of IPG in the other conditions was not due to a release from refractoriness at sites central to the auditory nerve. Experiment 3 measured thresholds at three IPGs, which were less than, equal to, and more than one half of the interval between successive pulses. Thresholds were lowest at the intermediate IPG. The results of all experiments could be fit by a linear model consisting of a lowpass filter based on the function relating threshold to the frequency of sinusoidal electrical stimulation. The data and model have implications for reducing the power consumption of cochlear implants.


Human Brain Mapping | 2005

Automated post-hoc noise cancellation tool for audio recordings acquired in an MRI scanner

Rhodri Cusack; Nick Cumming; Daniel Bor; Dennis Norris; Johannes Lyzenga

There are several types of experiment in which it is useful to have subjects speak overtly in a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner, including those studying the articulatory apparatus and the neural basis of speech production, and fMRI experiments in which speech is used as a response modality. Although it is relatively easy to record sound from the bore, it can be difficult to hear the speech over the very loud acoustic noise from the scanner. This is particularly a problem during echo‐planar imaging, which is usually used for fMRI. We present a post‐hoc sound cancellation algorithm, and describe a Windows‐based tool that implements it. The tool is fast and operates with minimal user intervention. We evaluate cancellation performance in terms of the improvement in signal‐to‐noise ratio, and investigate the effect of the recording medium. A substantial improvement in audibility was obtained. Hum. Brain Mapping 24:299–304, 2005.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2002

A speech enhancement scheme incorporating spectral expansion evaluated with simulated loss of frequency selectivity

Johannes Lyzenga; Joost M. Festen; Tammo Houtgast

Hearing-impaired listeners often suffer from supra-threshold speech perception deficits. One such deficit is reduced frequency selectivity. We applied a speech enhancement scheme that incorporated spectral expansion in an attempt to reduce the effects of this deficit. The speech processing could contain up to three stages, a first in which the peak-valley ratio of the speech spectrum was enlarged to counteract the broadening of the auditory filtering, and a second in which the overall speech spectrum was modified to counteract the effects of upward-spread-of-masking, using a linear filter. The third stage was a noise suppression stage, applied before the spectral enhancement. The effectiveness of the speech processing with and without noise suppression was evaluated for various parameter settings by measuring the speech reception threshold (SRT) in noise, i.e., the signal-to-noise ratio at which listeners repeat 50% of presented sentences correctly. We used normal-hearing subjects. To simulate the loss of frequency selectivity we applied spectral smearing to the stimuli presented to the subjects. The speech material of the SRT tests was mixed with the noise before processing, and, when present, the smearing was applied last. The results indicated that for one specific parameter setting the SRT values decreased (i.e., improved) by approximately 1 dB when incorporating the spectral expansion together with the linear filtering. Employing either of these two stages separately did not improve the SRT. The application of the noise suppression stage did not further improve the SRT. A pilot study using hearing-impaired listeners showed more promising results for a female than for a male speaker.


Hearing Research | 2005

Dynamic aspects of the continuity illusion: perception of level and of the depth, rate, and phase of modulation

Johannes Lyzenga; Robert P. Carlyon; Brian C. J. Moore

The perception of modulation of a tone interrupted by a noise burst was investigated. The tone and its modulation were perceived as continuing through the noise. In experiment 1, subjects rated the similarity of an uninterrupted tone and a tone interrupted by noise, in terms of the perceived level and modulation depth of the sinusoidal carrier. The values of these parameters in the central portion of the uninterrupted tone were systematically varied. Both amplitude and frequency modulation (AM and FM) were used. The results indicated that the perceived level and modulation depth of the carrier did not change greatly during the noise burst. When the modulation rate differed before and after the noise burst, the modulation-rate transition was perceived to occur near the end of the noise burst for the FM stimuli. Hence, for these stimuli, the continuity illusion appears to be dominated by the portion of the tone before, rather than after, the interruption. Results for the AM stimuli showed a non-significant trend in the same direction. Experiment 2 used forced-choice tasks to evaluate the ability to detect a change in the ongoing phase of AM and FM following interruption by a noise burst. The results confirmed earlier findings for FM tones, and extended them to AM tones, showing that listeners lost track of the phase of the modulation, even though the modulation was perceived as continuous.


International Journal of Audiology | 2013

Evaluation of the preliminary auditory profile test battery in an international multi-centre study

Thamar E. M. van Esch; Birger Kollmeier; Matthias Vormann; Johannes Lyzenga; Tammo Houtgast; Mathias Hällgren; Birgitta Larsby; Sheetal Athalye; Mark E. Lutman; Wouter A. Dreschler

Abstract Objective: This paper describes the composition and international multi-centre evaluation of a battery of tests termed the preliminary auditory profile. It includes measures of loudness perception, listening effort, speech perception, spectral and temporal resolution, spatial hearing, self-reported disability and handicap, and cognition. Clinical applicability and comparability across different centres are investigated. Design: Headphone tests were conducted in five centres divided over four countries. Effects of test-retest, ear, and centre were investigated. Results for normally-hearing (NH) and hearing-impaired (HI) listeners are presented. Study sample: Thirty NH listeners aged 19–39 years, and 72 HI listeners aged 22–91 years with a broad range of hearing losses were included. Results: Test-retest reliability was generally good and there were very few right/left ear effects. Results of all tests were comparable across centres for NH listeners after baseline correction to account for necessary differences between test materials. For HI listeners, results were comparable across centres for the language-independent tests. Conclusions: The auditory profile forms a clinical test battery that is applicable in four different languages. Even after baseline correction, differences between test materials have to be taken into account when interpreting results of language-dependent tests in HI listeners.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2003

The effects of real and illusory glides on pure-tone frequency discrimination

Johannes Lyzenga; Robert P. Carlyon; Brian C. J. Moore

Experiment 1 measured pure-tone frequency difference limens (DLs) at 1 and 4 kHz. The stimuli had two steady-state portions, which differed in frequency for the target. These portions were separated by a middle section of varying length, which consisted of a silent gap, a frequency glide, or a noise burst (conditions: gap, glide, and noise, respectively). The noise burst created an illusion of the tone continuing through the gap. In the first condition, the stimuli had an overall duration of 500 ms. In the second condition, stimuli had a fixed 50-ms middle section, and the overall duration was varied. DLs were lower for the glide than for the gap condition, consistent with the idea that the auditory system contains a mechanism specific for the detection of dynamic changes. DLs were generally lower for the noise than for the gap condition, suggesting that this mechanism extracts information from an illusory glide. In a second experiment, pure-tone frequency direction-discrimination thresholds were measured using similar stimuli as for the first experiment. For this task, the type of the middle section hardly affected the thresholds, suggesting that the frequency-change detection mechanism does not facilitate the identification of the direction of frequency changes.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1999

Center frequency modulation detection for harmonic complexes resembling vowel formants and its interference by off-frequency maskers

Johannes Lyzenga; Robert P. Carlyon

Vowels are characterized by peaks in their spectral envelopes: the formants. To gain insight into the perception of speech as well as into the basic abilities of the ear, sensitivity to modulations in the positions of these formants is investigated. Frequency modulation detection thresholds (FMTs) were measured for the center frequency of formantlike harmonic complexes in the absence and in the presence of simultaneous off-frequency formants (maskers). Both the signals and the maskers were harmonic complexes which were band-pass filtered with a triangular spectral envelope, on a log-log scale, into either a LOW (near 500 Hz), a MID (near 1500 Hz), or a HIGH region (near 3000 Hz). They had a duration of 250 ms, and either an 80- or a 240-Hz fundamental. The modulation rate was 5 Hz for the signals and 10 Hz for the maskers. A pink noise background was presented continuously. In a first experiment no maskers were used. The measured FMTs were roughly two times larger than previously reported just-noticeable differences for formant frequency. In a second experiment, no significant differences were found between the FMTs in the absence of maskers and those in the presence of stationary (i.e., nonfrequency modulated) maskers. However, under many conditions the FMTs were increased by the presence of simultaneous modulated maskers. These results indicate that frequency modulation detection interference (FMDI) can exist for formantlike complex tones. The FMDI data could be divided into two groups. For stimuli characterized by a steep (200-dB/oct) slope, it was found that the size of the FMDI depended on which cues were used for detecting the signal and masker modulations. For stimuli with shallow (50-dB/oct) slopes, the FMDI was reduced when the signal and the masker had widely differing fundamentals, implying that the fundamental information is extracted before the interference occurs.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2005

Estimating cochlear‐filter shapes, temporal‐window width and compression from tone‐sweep detection in spectral and temporal noise gaps

Gaston Hilkhuysen; Tammo Houtgast; Johannes Lyzenga

A test, designed for naive listeners, measured tone‐sweep detection in noise with either spectral or temporal gaps. For normal‐hearing (NH) listeners, detections in spectral gaps depended on level, which can be explained from Outer‐Hair‐Cell (OHC) activity. At low levels, OHC activity increased frequency‐selectivity by amplifying the signal in the spectral gap, improving the signal‐to‐noise ratio (SNR). Relative to the broad passive cochlear filter, OHC activity decreased with rising signal levels. In consequence, SNRs decreased and detection deteriorated. Similarly, decreasing OHC activity may explain the observed level dependence of detection thresholds in temporal gaps. At low and high intensities, signal and noise were equally amplified. Detection was best at intermediate levels when the low‐level signal in the temporal gap was amplified more than the high‐level noise. All effects are modeled using a one‐parameter time window with decaying‐exponential shape preceded by a simplified dual‐resonance non‐...


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2005

Detection, direction discrimination, and off-frequency interference of center-frequency modulations and glides for vowel formants.

Johannes Lyzenga; Robert P. Carlyon

Vowels are mainly classified by the positions of peaks in their frequency spectra, the formants. For normal-hearing subjects, change detection and direction discrimination were measured for linear glides in the center frequency (CF) of formantlike sounds. A CF rove was used to prevent subjects from using either the start or end points of the glides as cues. In addition, change detection and starting-phase (start-direction) discrimination were measured for similar stimuli with a sinusoidal 5-Hz formant-frequency modulation. The stimuli consisted of single formants generated using a number of different stimulus parameters including fundamental frequency, spectral slope, frequency region, and position of the formant relative to the harmonic spectrum. The change detection thresholds were in good agreement with the predictions of a model which analyzed and combined the effects of place-of-excitation and temporal cues. For most stimuli, thresholds were approximately equal for change detection and start-direction discrimination. Exceptions were found for stimuli that consisted of only one or two harmonics. In a separate experiment, it was shown that change detection and start-direction discrimination of linear and sinusoidal formant-frequency modulations were impaired by off-frequency frequency-modulated interferers. This frequency modulation detection interference was larger for formants with shallow than for those with steep spectral slopes.


International Journal of Audiology | 2015

Relations between psychophysical measures of spatial hearing and self-reported spatial-hearing abilities.

T van Esch; Mark E. Lutman; Matthias Vormann; Johannes Lyzenga; Mathias Hällgren; Birgitta Larsby; Sheetal Athalye; Tammo Houtgast; Birger Kollmeier; Wouter A. Dreschler

Abstract Objective: The aim of the present study was to investigate how well the virtual psychophysical measures of spatial hearing from the preliminary auditory profile predict self-reported spatial-hearing abilities. Design: Virtual spatial-hearings tests (conducted unaided, via headphones) and a questionnaire were administered in five centres in Germany, the Netherlands, Sweden, and the UK. Correlations and stepwise linear regression models were calculated among a group of hearing-impaired listeners. Study sample: Thirty normal-hearing listeners aged 19–39 years, and 72 hearing-impaired listeners aged 22–91 years with a broad range of hearing losses, including asymmetrical and mixed hearing losses. Results: Several significant correlations (between 0.24 and 0.54) were found between results of virtual psychophysical spatial-hearing tests and self-reported localization abilities. Stepwise linear regression analyses showed that the minimum audible angle (MAA) test was a significant predictor for self-reported localization abilities (5% extra explained variance), and the spatial speech reception threshold (SRT) benefit test for self-reported listening to speech in spatial situations (6% extra explained variance). Conclusions: The MAA test and spatial SRT benefit test are indicative measures of everyday binaural functioning. The binaural SRT benefit test was not found to predict self-reported spatial-hearing abilities.

Collaboration


Dive into the Johannes Lyzenga's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mark E. Lutman

University of Southampton

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robert P. Carlyon

Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sheetal Athalye

University of Southampton

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Joost M. Festen

VU University Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge