Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Jonathan Haidt is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Jonathan Haidt.


Psychological Review | 2001

The Emotional Dog and its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment

Jonathan Haidt

Research on moral judgment has been dominated by rationalist models, in which moral judgment is thought to be caused by moral reasoning. The author gives 4 reasons for considering the hypothesis that moral reasoning does not cause moral judgment; rather, moral reasoning is usually a post hoc construction, generated after a judgment has been reached. The social intuitionist model is presented as an alternative to rationalist models. The model is a social model in that it deemphasizes the private reasoning done by individuals and emphasizes instead the importance of social and cultural influences. The model is an intuitionist model in that it states that moral judgment is generally the result of quick, automatic evaluations (intuitions). The model is more consistent that rationalist models with recent findings in social, cultural, evolutionary, and biological psychology, as well as in anthropology and primatology.


Trends in Cognitive Sciences | 2002

How (and where) does moral judgment work

Joshua D. Greene; Jonathan Haidt

Moral psychology has long focused on reasoning, but recent evidence suggests that moral judgment is more a matter of emotion and affective intuition than deliberate reasoning. Here we discuss recent findings in psychology and cognitive neuroscience, including several studies that specifically investigate moral judgment. These findings indicate the importance of affect, although they allow that reasoning can play a restricted but significant role in moral judgment. They also point towards a preliminary account of the functional neuroanatomy of moral judgment, according to which many brain areas make important contributions to moral judgment although none is devoted specifically to it.


Personality and Individual Differences | 1994

Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors

Jonathan Haidt; Clark McCauley; Paul Rozin

We describe the development of a reliable measure of individual differences in disgust sensitivity. The 32-item Disgust Scale includes 2 true-false and 2 disgust-rating items for each of 7 domains of disgust elicitors (food, animals, body products, sex, body envelope violations, death, and hygiene) and for a domain of magical thinking (via similarity and contagion) that cuts across the 7 domains of elicitors. Correlations with other scales provide initial evidence of convergent and discriminant validity: the Disgust Scale correlates moderately with Sensation Seeking (r= - 0.46) and with Fear of Death (r= 0.39), correlates weakly with Neuroticism (r = 0.23) and Psychoticism (r= - 0.25), and correlates negligibly with Self-Monitoring and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Extraversion and Lie scales. Females score higher than males on the Disgust Scale. We suggest that the 7 domains of disgust elicitors all have in common that they remind us of our animality and, especially, of our mortality. Thus we see disgust as a defensive emotion that maintains and emphasizes the line between human and animal.


Cognition & Emotion | 1999

Social Functions of Emotions at Four Levels of Analysis

Dacher Keltner; Jonathan Haidt

In this paper we integrate claims and ® ndings concerning the social functions of emotions at the individual, dyadic, group, and cultural levels of analysis. Across levels of analysis theorists assume that emotions solve problems important to social relationships in the context of ongoing interactions. Theorists diverge, however, in their assumptions about the origins, de® ning characteristics, and consequences of emotions, and in their preferred forms of data. We illustrate the differences and compatibilities among these levels of analysis for the speci® c case of embarrassment. We close by suggesting research strategies that incorporate a social-functional perspective.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 1993

Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog ?

Jonathan Haidt; Silvia Helena Koller; Maria da Graça Bompastor Borges Dias

Are disgusting or disrespectful actions judged to be moral violations, even when they are harmless? Stories about victimless yet offensive actions (such as cleaning ones toilet with a flag) were presented to Brazilian and U.S. adults and children of high and low socioeconomic status (N = 360). Results show that college students at elite universities judged these stories to be matters of social convention or of personal preference. Most other Ss, especially in Brazil, took a moralizing stance toward these actions. For these latter Ss, moral judgments were better predicted by affective reactions than by appraisals of harmfulness. Results support the claims of cultural psychology (R.A. Shweder, 1991a) and suggest that cultural norms and culturally shaped emotions have a substantial impact on the domain of morality and the process of moral judgment. Suggestions are made for building cross-culturally valid models of moral judgment.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 1999

The CAD triad hypothesis: a mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity).

Paul Rozin; Laura Lowery; Sumio Imada; Jonathan Haidt

It is proposed that 3 emotions--contempt, anger, and disgust--are typically elicited, across cultures, by violations of 3 moral codes proposed by R. A. Shweder and his colleagues (R. A. Shweder, N. C. Much, M. Mahapatra, & L. Park, 1997). The proposed alignment links anger to autonomy (individual rights violations), contempt to community (violation of communal codes including hierarchy), and disgust to divinity (violations of purity-sanctity). This is the CAD triad hypothesis. Students in the United States and Japan were presented with descriptions of situations that involve 1 of the types of moral violations and asked to assign either an appropriate facial expression (from a set of 6) or an appropriate word (contempt, anger, disgust, or their translations). Results generally supported the CAD triad hypothesis. Results were further confirmed by analysis of facial expressions actually made by Americans to the descriptions of these situations.


Review of General Psychology | 2005

What (and Why) Is Positive Psychology

Shelly L. Gable; Jonathan Haidt

Positive psychology is the study of the conditions and processes that contribute to the flourishing or optimal functioning of people, groups, and institutions. In this brief introduction, the authors give examples of current work in positive psychology and try to explain why the positive psychology movement has grown so quickly in just 5 years. They suggest that it filled a need: It guided researchers to understudied phenomena. The authors close by addressing some criticisms and shortcomings of positive psychology, such as the relative lack of progress in studying positive institutions.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2011

Mapping the Moral Domain

Jesse Graham; Brian A. Nosek; Jonathan Haidt; Ravi Iyer; Spassena Koleva; Peter H. Ditto

The moral domain is broader than the empathy and justice concerns assessed by existing measures of moral competence, and it is not just a subset of the values assessed by value inventories. To fill the need for reliable and theoretically grounded measurement of the full range of moral concerns, we developed the Moral Foundations Questionnaire on the basis of a theoretical model of 5 universally available (but variably developed) sets of moral intuitions: Harm/Care, Fairness/Reciprocity, Ingroup/Loyalty, Authority/Respect, and Purity/Sanctity. We present evidence for the internal and external validity of the scale and the model, and in doing so we present new findings about morality: (a) Comparative model fitting of confirmatory factor analyses provides empirical justification for a 5-factor structure of moral concerns; (b) convergent/discriminant validity evidence suggests that moral concerns predict personality features and social group attitudes not previously considered morally relevant; and (c) we establish pragmatic validity of the measure in providing new knowledge and research opportunities concerning demographic and cultural differences in moral intuitions. These analyses provide evidence for the usefulness of Moral Foundations Theory in simultaneously increasing the scope and sharpening the resolution of psychological views of morality.


Archive | 2003

Flourishing : positive psychology and the life well-lived

Corey L. M. Keyes; Jonathan Haidt

Flourishing Under Fire: Resilience as a Prototype of Challenged Thriving-Carol Ryff and Burton Singer Turning Points as Opportunities for Personal Growth-Elaine Wethington Optimism and Flourishing-Chris Peterson and Edward Chang The Constructing of Meaning through Vital Engagement-Jeanne Nakamura and Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi Personal Goals, Life Meaning, and Virtue: Wellsprings of a Positive Life-Robert Emmons Toward a Positive Psychology of Relationships-Harry Reis and Shelly Gable Creativity and Genius-Vincent Cassandro and Dean Simonton Work, Play, and Eating On Making More of More Moments in Life-Amy Wrzesniewski, Paul Rozin, and Gwen Bennett Well-Being in the Workplace: A Review of the Gallup Studies-James Harter, Frank Schmidt, and Corey L. M. Keyes Doing Well by Doing Good: The Health Benefits of Community Participation-Jane Allyn Piliavin Wisdom: A Meta-Heuristic Guiding the Conduct of Life-Paul Baltes and Alexandra Freund Elevation and the Positive Psychology of Morality-Jonathan Haidt Complete Mental Health: An Agenda for the 21st Century-Corey L. M. Keyes


Daedalus | 2004

Intuitive ethics: how innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues

Jonathan Haidt; Craig Joseph

maps embellished with fantastical beasts, sixteenth-century wonder chambers 1⁄2lled with natural and technological marvels, even late-twentieth-century supermarket tabloids–all attest to the human fascination with things that violate our basic ideas about reality. The study of morality and culture is therefore an intrinsically fascinating topic. People have created moralities as divergent as those of Nazis and Quakers, headhunters and Jains. And yet, when we look closely at the daily lives of people in divergent cultures, we can 1⁄2nd elements that arise in nearly all of them– for example, reciprocity, loyalty, respect for (some) authority, limits on physical harm, and regulation of eating and sexuality. What are we to make of this pattern of similarity within profound difference? Social scientists have traditionally taken two approaches. The empiricist approach posits that moral knowledge, moral beliefs, moral action, and all the other stuff of morality are learned in childhood. There is no moral faculty or moral anything else built into the human mind, although there may be some innate learning mechanisms that enable the acquisition of later knowledge. To the extent that there are similarities across cultures, they arise because all cultures face similar problems (e.g., how to divide power and resources, care for children, and resolve disputes) for which they have often developed similar solutions. The nativist approach, on the other hand, holds that knowledge about such issues as fairness, harm, and respect for authority has been built into the human mind by evolution. All children who are raised in a reasonable environment will come to develop these ideas, even if they are not taught by adults. To the extent that there are differences across cultures, they arise because of local variation in the implementation of universal moral

Collaboration


Dive into the Jonathan Haidt's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jesse Graham

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ravi Iyer

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Peter H. Ditto

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Spassena Koleva

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Paul Rozin

University of Pennsylvania

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dacher Keltner

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Craig Joseph

Northwestern University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge