Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Joris E. Coppens is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Joris E. Coppens.


Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics | 2009

Straylight in the human eye: testing objectivity and optical character of the psychophysical measurement.

T. Berg; Luuk Franssen; Joris E. Coppens

The point spread function or PSF of the human eye encompasses hugely different domains: a small‐angle, high‐intensity domain, called the ‘PSF core’, and a large‐angle, low‐intensity domain, usually referred to as ‘straylight’. The first domain can be assessed by available double‐pass or other optical techniques. For the second domain psychophysical techniques have been developed, in particular the Compensation Comparison or CC technique, recently made available for clinical application in the C‐Quant instrument. We address the question of whether the psychophysical technique gives measures of straylight that are compatible with those made by optical methods. With a small adaptation the CC method can be used to assess straylight from physical light scattering samples, instead of straylight in the eye, using the same psychophysics, but without interference from the ocular straylight. The light scattered by each of seven light‐scattering samples, encompassing the range of straylight values observed in human eyes, was measured by two optical methods and by the psychophysical technique. The results showed that the optical and psychophysical measurements for the seven samples were almost identical.


Neuropsychology (journal) | 2007

Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents.

M.V. de Jonge; Chantal Kemner; E.H.F. de Haan; Joris E. Coppens; T. J. T. P. van den Berg; H. van Engeland

The authors assessed visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) and their parents. The authors used tasks for contrast sensitivity, motion, and form perception to test visual processing occurring relatively early and late in the magnocellular-dorsal and parvocellular-ventral pathways. No deficits were found in contrast sensitivity for low or high spatial frequencies or for motion or form perception between individuals with PDD in comparison with a matched control group. Individuals with PDD performed equally with or better than controls on motion detection tasks. In addition, the authors did not find differences on any of the tasks between parents of the PDD group and matched control parents. These results indicate that high-functioning individuals with PDD and their parents are able to process visual stimuli that rely on early or late processing in the magnocellular-dorsal and parvocellular-ventral pathways as well as controls.


Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery | 2006

Simulating the straylight effects of cataracts

Gerard C. de Wit; Luuk Franssen; Joris E. Coppens; T. Berg

PURPOSE: To study the additional straylight falling on the retina (retinal straylight) caused by cataract and find commercially available filters to simulate the cataract straylight effects. SETTING: Research laboratory. METHODS: The retinal straylight addition of cataract was derived from straylight parameter data in the literature. The scattering characteristics of cataract‐simulating filters were measured using a scatterometer. RESULTS: The straylight addition due to cataract follows a power law as a function of angle with power of −2.12 and straylight parameter log values of up to 1.6 for relatively mild cataract cases. Of the commercial filters that were tested, the Tiffen Black Pro Mist (BPM) filters resembled the straylight characteristics of cataracts fairly well. The filters had a limited effect on visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, which was also found for early cataracts. The BPM 2 followed a power law as a function of angle with power of approximately −2.21 and straylight log values of 1.12. CONCLUSIONS: The BPM 2 filter is a good early‐cataract‐simulating filter. Stacking such filters is a good way to increase the cataract density. A drawback is that the BPM 2 filter has a transmission of 66% so stacking filters reduces the overall transmission significantly.


Journal of Biomedical Optics | 2006

Reliability of the compensation comparison stray-light measurement method

Joris E. Coppens; Luuk Franssen; L.J. van Rijn; T. Berg

The compensation comparison (CC) method is a psychophysical technique to measure retinal stray light. It uses a two alternative forced choice (2AFC) measurement paradigm. The 25 binary (0 and 1) responses resulting from the 2AFC test are analyzed using maximum likelihood estimates. The likelihood function is used to give two quantities: the most likely stray-light level of the eye under investigation, and the accuracy of this estimate [called expected standard deviation (ESD)]. The CC method is used in 2422 subjects of the GLARE study. Each eye is tested twice to allow analysis of measurement repeatability. Furthermore, the large amount of responses is used to evaluate the shape of the psychometric function, for which a mathematical model is used. The shape of the psychometric function found by averaging the 0 and 1 responses fit well to the model function. Data sorted according to ESD show differences in the shape of the psychometric function between good and bad observers. These different shapes for the psychometric function are used to reanalyze the data, but the stray-light results remain virtually identical. ESD proves to be an efficient tool to detect unreliable measurements. In clinical practice, ESD may be used to decide whether to repeat a measurement.


Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology#R##N#Encyclopedia of the Eye | 2010

Ocular Media Clarity and Straylight

T. J. T. P. van den Berg; Luuk Franssen; Joris E. Coppens

The optical media of the human eye contain several errors, and light scattering is one of them. Light scattering results in the spreading of light perceived around bright light sources and complaints about glare, hazy vision, etc. This phenomenon is called straylight and, by international standards, is used as the definition for disability glare. Straylight is recognized as an important aspect of quality of vision, assessed functionally by objective psychophysical means (compensation comparison, C-Quant instrument). Normal aging causes straylight to increase twofold by the age of 65. In pathology, straylight can be increased by a factor of 10 or more, constituting a highly disabling condition.


Journal of Biomedical Optics | 2006

Reliability of the compensation comparison method for measuring retinal stray light studied using Monte-Carlo simulations.

Joris E. Coppens; Luuk Franssen; T. Berg

Recently the psychophysical compensation comparison method was developed for routine measurement of retinal stray light. The subjects responses to a series of two-alternative-forced-choice trials are analyzed using a maximum-likelihood (ML) approach assuming some fixed shape for the psychometric function (PF). This study evaluates the reliability of the method using Monte-Carlo simulations. Various sampling strategies were investigated, including the two-phase sampling strategy that is used in a commercially available instrument. Results are given for the effective dynamic range and measurement accuracy. The effect of a mismatch of the shape of the PF of an observer and the fixed shape used in the ML analysis was analyzed. Main outcomes are that the two-phase sampling scheme gives good precision (Standard deviation = 0.07 logarithmic units on average) for estimation of the stray light value. Bias is virtually zero. Furthermore, a reliability index was derived from the responses and found to be effective.


eurographics | 2009

Temporal Glare: Real-Time Dynamic Simulation of the Scattering in the Human Eye

Tobias Ritschel; Matthias Ihrke; Jeppe Revall Frisvad; Joris E. Coppens; Karol Myszkowski; Hans-Peter Seidel

Glare is a consequence of light scattered within the human eye when looking at bright light sources. This effect can be exploited for tone mapping since adding glare to the depiction of high‐dynamic range (HDR) imagery on a low‐dynamic range (LDR) medium can dramatically increase perceived contrast. Even though most, if not all, subjects report perceiving glare as a bright pattern that fluctuates in time, up to now it has only been modeled as a static phenomenon. We argue that the temporal properties of glare are a strong means to increase perceived brightness and to produce realistic and attractive renderings of bright light sources. Based on the anatomy of the human eye, we propose a model that enables real‐time simulation of dynamic glare on a GPU. This allows an improved depiction of HDR images on LDR media for interactive applications like games, feature films, or even by adding movement to initially static HDR images. By conducting psychophysical studies, we validate that our method improves perceived brightness and that dynamic glare‐renderings are often perceived as more attractive depending on the chosen scene.


Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | 2014

Sleep spindle and slow wave frequency reflect motor skill performance in primary school-age children

Rebecca G. Astill; Giovanni Piantoni; Roy J.E.M. Raymann; J.C. Vis; Joris E. Coppens; Matthew P. Walker; Robert Stickgold; Ysbrand D. van der Werf; Eus J. W. Van Someren

Background and Aim: The role of sleep in the enhancement of motor skills has been studied extensively in adults. We aimed to determine involvement of sleep and characteristics of spindles and slow waves in a motor skill in children. Hypothesis: We hypothesized sleep-dependence of skill enhancement and an association of interindividual differences in skill and sleep characteristics. Methods: 30 children (19 females, 10.7 ± 0.8 years of age; mean ± SD) performed finger sequence tapping tasks in a repeated-measures design spanning 4 days including 1 polysomnography (PSG) night. Initial and delayed performance were assessed over 12 h of wake; 12 h with sleep; and 24 h with wake and sleep. For the 12 h with sleep, children were assigned to one of three conditions: modulation of slow waves and spindles was attempted using acoustic perturbation, and compared to yoked and no-sound control conditions. Analyses: Mixed effect regression models evaluated the association of sleep, its macrostructure and spindles and slow wave parameters with initial and delayed speed and accuracy. Results and Conclusions: Children enhance their accuracy only over an interval with sleep. Unlike previously reported in adults, children enhance their speed independent of sleep, a capacity that may to be lost in adulthood. Individual differences in the dominant frequency of spindles and slow waves were predictive for performance: children performed better if they had less slow spindles, more fast spindles and faster slow waves. On the other hand, overnight enhancement of accuracy was most pronounced in children with more slow spindles and slower slow waves, i.e., the ones with an initial lower performance. Associations of spindle and slow wave characteristics with initial performance may confound interpretation of their involvement in overnight enhancement. Slower frequencies of characteristic sleep events may mark slower learning and immaturity of networks involved in motor skills.


Journal of The Optical Society of America A-optics Image Science and Vision | 2004

Corneal surface reconstruction algorithm that uses Zernike polynomial representation

Victor Arni D. P. Sicam; Joris E. Coppens; T. Berg; Rob G. L. Van Der Heijde

We developed an algorithm that directly determines Zernike coefficients for the corneal anterior surface derived from the reflection image of a stimulus with pseudorandom encoding. This algorithm does not need to include calculation of corneal height maps. The numerical performance of the algorithm is good. It has the potential of determining corneal shape with submicrometer accuracy in obtaining Zernike coefficients. When applied to real eye measurements the accuracy of the procedure will be limited by the topographer that is used.


International Journal of Psychophysiology | 2013

Modulation of gamma and spindle-range power by slow oscillations in scalp sleep EEG of children

Giovanni Piantoni; Rebecca G. Astill; Roy Raymann; J.C. Vis; Joris E. Coppens; Eus J. W. Van Someren

Deep sleep is characterized by slow waves of electrical activity in the cerebral cortex. They represent alternating down states and up states of, respectively, hyperpolarization with accompanying neuronal silence and depolarization during which neuronal firing resumes. The up states give rise to faster oscillations, notably spindles and gamma activity which appear to be of major importance to the role of sleep in brain function and cognition. Unfortunately, while spindles are easily detectable, gamma oscillations are of very small amplitude. No previous sleep study has succeeded in demonstrating modulations of gamma power along the time course of slow waves in human scalp EEG. As a consequence, progress in our understanding of the functional role of gamma modulation during sleep has been limited to animal studies and exceptional human studies, notably those of intracranial recordings in epileptic patients. Because high synaptic density, which peaks some time before puberty depending on the brain region (Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997), generates oscillations of larger amplitude, we considered that the best chance to demonstrate a modulation of gamma power by slow wave phase in regular scalp sleep EEG would be in school-aged children. Sleep EEG was recorded in 30 healthy children (aged 10.7 ± 0.8 years; mean ± s.d.). Time-frequency analysis was applied to evaluate the time course of spectral power along the development of a slow wave. Moreover, we attempted to modify sleep architecture and sleep characteristics through automated acoustic stimulation coupled to the occurrence of slow waves in one subset of the children. Gamma power increased on the rising slope and positive peak of the slow wave. Gamma and spindle activity is strongly suppressed during the negative peak. There were no differences between the groups who received and did not receive acoustic stimulation in the sleep parameters and slow wave-locked time-frequency analysis. Our findings show, for the first time in scalp EEG in humans, that gamma activity is associated with the up-going slope and peak of the slow wave. We propose that studies in children provide a uniquely feasible opportunity to conduct investigations into the role of gamma during sleep.

Collaboration


Dive into the Joris E. Coppens's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Luuk Franssen

Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

T. Berg

Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bart H.W. te Lindert

Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bastiaan Kruijt

Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jessica Bruijel

Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

T. J. T. P. van den Berg

Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Thomas J. T. P. van den Berg

Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge