Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Jos G.M. Hendriksen is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Jos G.M. Hendriksen.


Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health | 2001

Neighbourhood level and individual level SES effects on child problem behaviour: a multilevel analysis

Ariane C. Kalff; Mariëlle Kroes; Johan S.H. Vles; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Frans Feron; Jean Steyaert; T M C B van Zeben; J. Jolles; J. van Os

OBJECTIVE This study examined whether neighbourhood level socioeconomic variables have an independent effect on reported child behaviour problems over and above the effect of individual level measures of socioeconomic status. DESIGN AND SETTING Multilevel analysis of cross sectional survey data relating individual level child behavioural problems and parental measures of socioeconomic status with neighbourhood level measures of socioeconomic deprivation in the city of Maastricht, the Netherlands. PARTICIPANTS Children born in the years 1990–1991 attending the second grade of normal kindergarten schools in the city of Maastricht, the Netherlands. Out of 1417 eligible 5–7 year olds, the parents of 734 children (51.8%) agreed to participate. MAIN RESULTS Child behaviour problems were more frequent in families of low parental occupation and education (F=14.51, df 3, 721, p<0.001; F=12.20, df 3, 721, p<0.001, respectively) and in families living in deprived neighbourhoods (F=13.26, df 2, 722, p<0.001). Multilevel random effects regression analysis showed that the effect of neighbourhood level deprivation remained after adjustment for individual level socioeconomic status (B over three levels of deprivation: 1.36; 95%CI=0.28, 2.45). CONCLUSIONS Living in a more deprived neighbourhood is associated with higher levels of child problem behaviour, irrespective of individual level socioeconomic status. The additional effect of the neighbourhood may be attributable to contextual variables such as the level of social cohesion among residents.


Brain and Cognition | 2004

Verbal fluency over time as a measure of automatic and controlled processing in children with ADHD

Petra P. M. Hurks; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Johan S.H. Vles; Ariane C. Kalff; Frans Feron; Mariëlle Kroes; T M C B van Zeben; Jean Steyaert; J. Jolles

The performance of ADHD children on semantic category fluency (SCF) versus initial letter fluency (ILF) tasks was examined. For each participant, word production was recorded for each 15-s time slice on each task. Performance on both fluency tasks was compared to test the hypothesis that children with ADHD are characterized by a performance deficit on the ILF task because performance on this task is less automated than performance on the SCF. Children classified with ADHD (N = 20) were compared to children with other psychopathology (N = 118) and healthy controls (N = 130). Results indicated that the groups could not be differentiated by the total number of words produced in 60 s in either fluency task. As hypothesized, a significant interaction of group by productivity over time by type of fluency task was found: ADHD children had more problems finding words in the first 15 s of the IFL than did children in the other two groups, and as compared with their performance on the SCF. Results were taken to indicate that children with ADHD symptoms show a delay in the development of automating skills for processing abstract verbal information.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 2001

Child Psychiatric Diagnoses in a Population of Dutch Schoolchildren Aged 6 to 8 Years

Mariëlle Kroes; Ariane C. Kalff; Alfons G. H. Kessels; Jean Steyaert; Frans Feron; Astrid J.W.G.M. Van Someren; Petra P. M. Hurks; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Thea M.C.B. Van Zeben; Nico Rozendaal; Inge F.A.M. Crolla; J. Troost; Jelle Jolles; Johan S.H. Vles

OBJECTIVE To determine the prevalence rates of child psychiatric diagnoses in a school-based population of children aged 6 to 8 years in the south of the province of Limburg (The Netherlands). METHOD In a two-stage design 1,317 children were screened with the Child Behavior Checklist. From 403 of these children, child psychiatric information was obtained with the Amsterdam Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (ADIKA, DSM-III-R/IV). Data were generalized to the responder group (n = 1,317) and to the entire cohort (N = 2,290). For the latter procedure, a prediction model was used to generalize ADIKA results to the nonresponders (n = 973). RESULTS Estimates of the prevalence of different ADIKA diagnoses in the responder group were quite comparable with those for the entire cohort. Twenty-four percent of the entire cohort met criteria for a single disorder, and 21.0% met criteria for two or more disorders. However, in only 5.7% of the cases parents did report a need for help. CONCLUSIONS Where other studies generalize psychiatric diagnoses to the responder group only, this report adds new information by generalizing the prevalence to a school-based cohort of children aged 6 to 8 years. These prevalence estimates are of importance with regard to the demand for care for child psychopathology.


Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology | 2006

Semantic category fluency versus initial letter fluency over 60 seconds as a measure of automatic and controlled processing in healthy school-aged children

Petra P. M. Hurks; Johan S.H. Vles; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Ariane C. Kalff; Frans Feron; Mariëlle Kroes; T M C B van Zeben; Jean Steyaert; J. Jolles

Verbal fluency was operationalized as the number of words produced in a restricted category (i.e., semantic category [SCF] and words beginning with a given letter [ILF]) in 60 seconds. Word production in the first 15 seconds of either type of fluency task was defined as a measure of automatic information processing, whereas word production in the remaining 45 seconds (in 15-second periods) was taken as a measure of controlled information processing. Data revealed that over 60 seconds healthy children aged 8.4–9.7 years (n = 91) produced significantly more words and less incorrect responses on the SCF task than on the ILF task. Although word production was a function of both type of task and time, it was highest in the initial time slice of either type of fluency and decreased as time on task increased. Finally, no sex differences were found for any measure of performance on either type of fluency task. In contrast, the level of occupational achievement of the caregiver (LOA) appeared to be a determinant of the child’s performance on either type of fluency task, indicating that LOA affects higher-order processes, such as the automation of newly learned verbal skills and effortful processing.


Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology | 2002

Neurocognitive performance of 5- and 6-year-old children who met criteria for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder at 18 months follow-up: results from a prospective population study.

Ariane C. Kalff; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Mariëlle Kroes; Johan S.H. Vles; Jean Steyaert; Frans Feron; Thea M.C.B. Van Zeben; Jelle Jolles

The aim of this prospective study was to examine whether neurocognitive performance of children aged 5–6 years distinguished children who were later diagnosed with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or “borderline ADHD” from children without ADHD after adjustment for behavioral measures and to examine the influence of comorbid psychopathology. Out of a general population of 1,317 children, 366 children were selected on the basis of their scores on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Eighteen months later, the parents were interviewed using a standardized child psychiatric interview: 33 children were classified as ADHD and 75 children as borderline ADHD, and there were 258 children without ADHD. Children with rated ADHD were significantly impaired on measures of visuomotor ability and working memory compared to children without ADHD after adjustment for CBCL results. The performance of borderline ADHD children was in between that of children with and without ADHD. In addition, 4 groups of children were analyzed: 9 ADHD, 24 ADHD with comorbid oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder (ODD/CD), 59 ODD/CD, and 274 controls. Children with rated comorbid ADHD and ODD/CD performed significantly worse on these tasks compared to children with rated ODD/CD and control children while they did not differ from ADHD children. Our results imply that neurocognitive measures can contribute to the early identification of ADHD with and without comorbid ODD/CD.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 2002

A Longitudinal Community Study: Do Psychosocial Risk Factors and Child Behavior Checklist Scores at 5 Years of Age Predict Psychiatric Diagnoses at a Later Age?

Mariëlle Kroes; Ariane C. Kalff; Jean Steyaert; Alfons G. H. Kessels; Frans Feron; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Thea M.C.B. Van Zeben; J. Troost; Jelle Jolles; Johan S.H. Vles

OBJECTIVE To examine the extent to which certain risk factors in 5- to 6-year-old children predict later psychopathology in a population-based sample of children from the province of Limburg in the south of the Netherlands. METHOD Of the 2,290 children of interest, 1,317 children were screened with the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and psychosocial risk factors for these children were collected. On the basis of the CBCL ratings, 403 children participated in the second stage in which, 1.5 years later, standardized child psychiatric information was obtained. Weighted logistic regression analyses were used to investigate predictors of psychopathology. RESULTS In separate analyses of specific types of child psychopathology, different risk factors emerged as significant. Low-level parental occupation and having foreign-born parents were predictive of conduct disorders, and living in a single-parent family and a having a life event were the most important predictors of mood and anxiety disorders. Furthermore, CBCL-based ratings at 5 to 6 years of age corresponded well with interview-defined diagnoses 1.5 years later. CONCLUSIONS Investigation of psychosocial risk factors and CBCL scores at the age of 5 to 6 years could be helpful in predicting child psychopathology and could help identify children at risk, in order to provide them with timely attention.


Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology | 2001

Factors affecting the relation between parental education as well as occupation and problem behaviour in Dutch 5- to 6-year-old children

Ariane C. Kalff; Mariëlle Kroes; Johan S.H. Vles; Hans Bosma; Frans Feron; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Jean Steyaert; T M C B van Zeben; I. F. A. M. Crolla; Jelle Jolles

Background: This study investigated whether problem behaviour in 5- to 6-year-old children is related to parental education and occupation. It also analysed the contribution of correlating factors to explain this association. Method: The Child Behaviour Checklist was administered to a large community sample of 1317 children who were in the 1st year of normal primary school in the Netherlands. Outcome measures were total problem score, and externalising and internalising scale scores. Results: Results of the logistic regression analyses indicated that higher rates of reported behaviour problems were significantly associated with low parental education and occupation. These associations were mediated by low maternal age at delivery and single-parent families. The number of children in a family and physical illness of the parents did not contribute to these associations. Conclusions: Parental education and occupation have a large impact on the mental health of young children. Psychosocial and biological factors are possible explanations for this phenomenon.


Journal of The International Neuropsychological Society | 2005

Speed, speed variability, and accuracy of information processing in 5 to 6-year-old children at risk of ADHD

Ariane C. Kalff; Leo M. J. de Sonneville; Petra P. M. Hurks; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Mariëlle Kroes; Frans Feron; Jean Steyaert; Thea M.C.B. Van Zeben; Johan S.H. Vles; Jelle Jolles

The early assessment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) children has mainly focused on the behavioral, social, and pre-academic impairments. This study examined whether 5 to 6-year-old children at risk of ADHD are characterized by information-processing deficits. By screening 1317 children in Southern Limburg (the Netherlands) with the Child Behavior Checklist, 363 were selected and underwent a computerized examination. Eighteen months later, standardized psychiatric information was obtained. Thirty-three ADHD children were compared with 75 borderline ADHD children, 122 pathological controls, and 133 healthy controls. ADHD and borderline ADHD children were slower and more variable in their processing speed on all tasks than children with no or other pathology. These differences were most pronounced for the divided and focused attention tasks. Furthermore, one measure of a state regulation deficit discriminated between groups. With regard to accuracy, only the proportion of misses on a go-no-go task was higher in the ADHD group than in the other groups. Evidence was found that ADHD is better seen as a continuum rather than a discrete category. Already at a young age, children at risk of ADHD show specific information-processing deficits. Deficits in time perception and/or energetic state control in children with ADHD may possibly account for subnormal task performance.


Epilepsy & Behavior | 2010

Nocturnal epileptiform EEG discharges, nocturnal epileptic seizures, and language impairments in children: Review of the literature

Geke M. Overvliet; René M.H. Besseling; Johan S.H. Vles; Paul A. M. Hofman; Walter H. Backes; M.H.J.A. van Hall; Sylvia Klinkenberg; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; Albert P. Aldenkamp

This review addresses the effect on language function of nocturnal epileptiform EEG discharges and nocturnal epileptic seizures in children. In clinical practice, language impairment is frequently reported in association with nocturnal epileptiform activity. Vice versa, nocturnal epileptiform EEG abnormalities are a common finding in children with specific language impairment. We suggest a spectrum that is characterized by nocturnal epileptiform activity and language impairment ranging from specific language impairment to rolandic epilepsy, nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy, electrical status epilepticus of sleep, and Landau-Kleffner syndrome. In this spectrum, children with specific language impairment have the best outcome, and children with electrical status epilepticus of sleep or Landau-Kleffner syndrome, the worst. The exact nature of this relationship and the factors causing this spectrum are unknown. We suggest that nocturnal epileptiform EEG discharges and nocturnal epileptic seizures during development will cause or contribute to diseased neuronal networks involving language. The diseased neuronal networks are less efficient compared with normal neuronal networks. This disorganization may cause language impairments.


European Journal of Paediatric Neurology | 2012

Cognitive effects of interictal epileptiform discharges in children

Saskia C.M. Ebus; Johan Arends; Jos G.M. Hendriksen; E. van der Horst; N. de la Parra; Ruben G.F. Hendriksen; E. Santegoeds; Paul Boon; B. Aldenkamp

Frequent interictal epileptiform discharges (IEDs) may have effects on cognition. We analysed a group of 182 children with different epilepsy syndromes as well as children with IEDs without observed seizures [corrected], with 24-h ambulatory EEG and cognitive tests. The IED index was estimated, in wakefulness and in sleep, as percentage of time in five categories (0%, <1%, 1-10%, ≥10-50% and ≥50%). IEDs were defined as spikes or spike-wave complexes, isolated or occurring serially (in runs) without evident clinical signs of a seizure. The IED categories were correlated to cognitive test results and epilepsy characteristics. The group of patients with diurnal IEDs in ≥10% of the EEG record showed impaired central information processing speed, short-term verbal memory and visual-motor integration. This effect was seen independently from other EEG-related and epilepsy-related characteristics, and independently from epilepsy syndrome diagnosis. The impact of the nocturnal IEDs was of less importance; only contributing partially to the slowing of central information processing speed. We conclude that frequent IEDs (in more than 10% of the record) in the awake EEG can impair cognitive performance in children. Whether children with a high diurnal spike frequency and low seizure frequency can benefit from antiepileptic treatment should be examined in controlled trials.

Collaboration


Dive into the Jos G.M. Hendriksen's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jelle Jolles

VU University Amsterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Erik H. Niks

Leiden University Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

N. Doorenweerd

Leiden University Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jean Steyaert

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge