Joshua M. Tybur
VU University Amsterdam
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Joshua M. Tybur.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2009
Joshua M. Tybur; Debra Lieberman; Vladas Griskevicius
What is the function of disgust? Whereas traditional models have suggested that disgust serves to protect the self or neutralize reminders of our animal nature, an evolutionary perspective suggests that disgust functions to solve 3 qualitatively different adaptive problems related to pathogen avoidance, mate choice, and social interaction. The authors investigated this 3-domain model of disgust across 4 studies and examined how sensitivity to these functional domains relates to individual differences in other psychological constructs. Consistent with their predictions, factor analyses demonstrated that disgust sensitivity partitions into domains related to pathogens, sexuality, and morality. Further, sensitivity to the 3 domains showed predictable differentiation based on sex, perceived vulnerability to disease, psychopathic tendencies, and Big 5 personality traits. In exploring these 3 domains of disgust, the authors introduce a new measure of disgust sensitivity. Appreciation of the functional heterogeneity of disgust has important implications for research on individual differences in disgust sensitivity, emotion, clinical impairments, and neuroscience.
Psychological Review | 2013
Joshua M. Tybur; Debra Lieberman; Robert Kurzban; Peter DeScioli
Interest in and research on disgust has surged over the past few decades. The field, however, still lacks a coherent theoretical framework for understanding the evolved function or functions of disgust. Here we present such a framework, emphasizing 2 levels of analysis: that of evolved function and that of information processing. Although there is widespread agreement that disgust evolved to motivate the avoidance of contact with disease-causing organisms, there is no consensus about the functions disgust serves when evoked by acts unrelated to pathogen avoidance. Here we suggest that in addition to motivating pathogen avoidance, disgust evolved to regulate decisions in the domains of mate choice and morality. For each proposed evolved function, we posit distinct information processing systems that integrate function-relevant information and account for the trade-offs required of each disgust system. By refocusing the discussion of disgust on computational mechanisms, we recast prior theorizing on disgust into a framework that can generate new lines of empirical and theoretical inquiry.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2011
Vladas Griskevicius; Joshua M. Tybur; Andrew W. Delton; Theresa E. Robertson
Why do some people take risks and live for the present, whereas others avoid risks and save for the future? The evolutionary framework of life history theory predicts that preferences for risk and delay in gratification should be influenced by mortality and resource scarcity. A series of experiments examined how mortality cues influenced decisions involving risk preference (e.g.,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2011
Jill M. Sundie; Douglas T. Kenrick; Vladas Griskevicius; Joshua M. Tybur; Kathleen D. Vohs; Daniel J. Beal
10 for sure vs. 50% chance of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2011
Vladas Griskevicius; Andrew W. Delton; Theresa E. Robertson; Joshua M. Tybur
20) and temporal discounting (e.g.,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2009
Vladas Griskevicius; Joshua M. Tybur; Steven W. Gangestad; Elaine F. Perea; Jenessa R. Shapiro; Douglas T. Kenrick
5 now vs.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2011
Vladas Griskevicius; Andrew W. Delton; Theresa E. Robertson; Joshua M. Tybur
10 later). The effect of mortality depended critically on whether people grew up in a relatively resource-scarce or resource-plentiful environment. For individuals who grew up relatively poor, mortality cues led them to value the present and gamble for big immediate rewards. Conversely, for individuals who grew up relatively wealthy, mortality cues led them to value the future and avoid risky gambles. Overall, mortality cues appear to propel individuals toward diverging life history strategies as a function of childhood socioeconomic status, suggesting important implications for how environmental factors influence economic decisions and risky behaviors.
Psychological Science | 2013
Vladas Griskevicius; Joshua M. Ackerman; Stephanie M. Cantú; Andrew W. Delton; Theresa E. Robertson; Jeffry A. Simpson; Melissa Emery Thompson; Joshua M. Tybur
Conspicuous consumption is a form of economic behavior in which self-presentational concerns override desires to obtain goods at bargain prices. Showy spending may be a social signal directed at potential mates. We investigated such signals by examining (a) which individuals send them, (b) which contexts trigger them, and (c) how observers interpret them. Three experiments demonstrated that conspicuous consumption is driven by men who are following a lower investment (vs. higher investment) mating strategy and is triggered specifically by short-term (vs. long-term) mating motives. A fourth experiment showed that observers interpret such signals accurately, with women perceiving men who conspicuously consume as being interested in short-term mating. Furthermore, conspicuous purchasing enhanced mens desirability as a short-term (but not as a long-term) mate. Overall, these findings suggest that flaunting status-linked goods to potential mates is not simply about displaying economic resources. Instead, conspicuous consumption appears to be part of a more precise signaling system focused on short-term mating. These findings contribute to an emerging literature on human life-history strategies.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2012
Vladas Griskevicius; Joshua M. Tybur; Joshua M. Ackerman; Andrew W. Delton; Theresa E. Robertson; Andrew Edward White
Why do some people have children early, whereas others delay reproduction? By considering the trade-offs between using ones resources for reproduction versus other tasks, the evolutionary framework of life history theory predicts that reproductive timing should be influenced by mortality and resource scarcity. A series of experiments examined how mortality cues influenced the desire to have children sooner rather than later. The effects of mortality depended critically on whether people grew up in a relatively resource-scarce or resource-plentiful environment. For individuals growing up relatively poor, mortality cues produced a desire to reproduce sooner--to want children now, even at the cost of furthering ones education or career. Conversely, for individuals growing up relatively wealthy, mortality cues produced a desire to delay reproduction--to further ones education or career before starting a family. Overall, mortality cues appear to shift individuals into different life history strategies as a function of childhood socioeconomic status, suggesting important implications for how environmental factors can influence fertility and family size.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B | 2011
Joshua M. Tybur; Steven W. Gangestad
Given the high costs of aggression, why have people evolved to act aggressively? Comparative biologists have frequently observed links between aggression, status, and mating in nonhuman animals. In this series of experiments, the authors examined the effects of status, competition, and mating motives on mens and womens aggression. For men, status motives increased direct aggression (face-to-face confrontation). Mens aggression was also boosted by mating motives, but only when observers were other men. For women, both status and mating motives increased indirect aggression (e.g., socially excluding the perpetrator). Although neither status nor mating motives increased womens direct aggression, women did become more directly aggressive when motivated to compete for scarce resources. These context- and sex-specific effects on human aggression contribute to a broader understanding of the functional nature of aggressive behavior.