Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Julie M. Estis is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Julie M. Estis.


Journal of Voice | 2011

Pitch-Matching Accuracy in Trained Singers and Untrained Individuals: The Impact of Musical Interference and Noise

Julie M. Estis; Ashli Dean-Claytor; Robert E. Moore; Thomas L. Rowell

The effects of musical interference and noise on pitch-matching accuracy were examined. Vocal training was explored as a factor influencing pitch-matching accuracy, and the relationship between pitch matching and pitch discrimination was examined. Twenty trained singers (TS) and 20 untrained individuals (UT) vocally matched tones in six conditions (immediate, four types of chords, noise). Fundamental frequencies were calculated, compared with the frequency of the target tone, and converted to semitone difference scores. A pitch discrimination task was also completed. TS showed significantly better pitch matching than UT across all conditions. Individual performances for UT were highly variable. Therefore, untrained participants were divided into two groups: 10 untrained accurate and 10 untrained inaccurate. Comparison of TS with untrained accurate individuals revealed significant differences between groups and across conditions. Compared with immediate vocal matching of target tones, pitch-matching accuracy was significantly reduced, given musical chord and noise interference unless the target tone was presented in the musical chord. A direct relationship between pitch matching and pitch discrimination was revealed. Across pitch-matching conditions, TS were consistently more accurate than UT. Pitch-matching accuracy diminished when auditory interference consisted of chords that did not contain the target tone and noise.


Journal of Voice | 2009

Effects of Increasing Time Delays on Pitch-Matching Accuracy in Trained Singers and Untrained Individuals

Julie M. Estis; Joana K. Coblentz; Robert E. Moore

Trained singers (TS) generally demonstrate accurate pitch matching, but this ability varies within the general population. Pitch-matching accuracy, given increasing silence intervals of 5, 15, and 25 seconds between target tones and vocal matches, was investigated in TS and untrained individuals. A relationship between pitch discrimination and pitch matching was also examined. Thirty-two females (20-30 years) were grouped based on individual vocal training and performance in an immediate pitch-matching task. Participants matched target pitches following time delays, and completed a pitch discrimination task, which required the classification of two tones as same or different. TS and untrained accurate participants performed comparably on all pitch-matching tasks, while untrained inaccurate participants performed significantly less accurately than the other two groups. Performances declined across groups as intervals of silence increased, suggesting degradation of pitch matching as pitch memory was taxed. A significant relationship between pitch discrimination and pitch matching was revealed across participants.


Journal of Child Language | 2015

Children with SLI Exhibit Delays Resolving Ambiguous Reference.

Julie M. Estis; Brenda L. Beverly

Fast mapping weaknesses in children with specific language impairment (SLI) may be explained by differences in disambiguation, mapping an unknown word to an unnamed object. The impact of language ability and linguistic stimulus on disambiguation was investigated. Sixteen children with SLI (8 preschool, 8 school-age) and sixteen typically developing age-matched children selected referents given familiar and unfamiliar object pairs in three ambiguous conditions: phonologically distinct word (PD), phonologically similar word (PS), no word (NW). Preschoolers with SLI did not disambiguate, unlike typically developing age-matched participants, who consistently selected unfamiliar objects given PD. School-age children with SLI disambiguated given PD. Delays in disambiguation for young children with SLI suggest limitations in processes that facilitate word learning for typically developing children. School-age children with SLI consistently selected familiar objects for PS, unlike typically developing children, suggesting differences in phonological activation for word learning.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2010

The effects of ear of stimulus presentation on pitch discrimination abilities of young adult females.

Ashley R. Gaal; Robert E. Moore; Julie M. Estis

Previous research has examined various factors that influence pitch discrimination abilities in normal‐hearing listeners [Deutsch (1970); (1978)], including the influence of spatial location of input stimuli. Separating the input location of interference tones from the initial (reference) and final (comparison) tones leads to better pitch discrimination accuracy (PDA) than when all tones are presented to the same ear, but the effects of the relationships between these three tone types have not been explored. This study examined the impact of ear of stimulation in 20 conditions: 5 containing no interference and 15 containing 4 interference tones. Twenty‐four non‐musician females (age 19–30) with normal hearing participated. Results showed that performance was significantly better when (a) no interference was present, (b) the comparison tone was presented contralaterally to reference and interference tones, (c) the comparison tone was presented to the left ear, and (d) the reference and comparison tones wer...


Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research | 2016

Short-Term Memory Performance in 7- and 8-Year-Old Children: The Relationship Between Phonological and Pitch Processing

Ashley Gaal Flagge; Julie M. Estis; Robert E. Moore

Purpose The relationship between short-term memory for phonology and pitch was explored by examining accuracy scores for typically developing children for 5 experimental tasks: immediate nonword repetition (NWR), nonword repetition with an 8-s silent interference (NWRS), pitch discrimination (PD), pitch discrimination with an 8-s silent interference (PDS), and pitch matching (PM). Method Thirty-six 7- and 8-year-old children (21 girls, 15 boys) with normal hearing, language, and cognition were asked to listen to and repeat nonsense words (NWR, NWRS), make a same versus different decision between 2 tones (PD, PDS), and listen to and then vocally reproduce a tone (PM). Results Results showed no significant correlations between tasks of phonological memory and tests of pitch memory, that participants scored significantly better on nonword repetition tasks than PD and PM tasks, and that participants performed significantly better on tasks with no silent interference. Discussion These findings suggest that, for typically developing children, pitch may be stored and rehearsed in a separate location than phonological information. Because of fundamental task differences, further research is needed to corroborate these data and determine the presence of developmental effects and neuroanatomical locations where a potential language/music overlap is occurring in children.


American Journal of Audiology | 2014

Digit Speech Recognition Threshold (SRT) in Children with Normal Hearing Ages 5–8 years

Ishara Ramkissoon; Julie M. Estis; Ashley Gaal Flagge

PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to compare digit stimuli to popular spondees when measuring speech recognition threshold (SRT) in children with normal hearing and to determine the influence of increment size using a descending method (ASHA, 1988). METHOD SRT was measured with paired digit stimuli (D-SRT) and pediatric word stimuli (W-SRT) using 2 step sizes (5 dB, 2 dB) in 30 typically developing children with normal hearing ages 5;0 (years;months) to 8;11. Pure-tone and SRT tests were administered in both ears. The D-SRT and W-SRT were compared to pure-tone average (PTA) thresholds per ear. RESULTS Analysis of variance revealed a significant interaction of test and step size: PTA was significantly lower when measured with 2-dB than with 5-dB increment; however, step size yielded no differences in D-SRT and W-SRT. Right ear thresholds across tests were significantly lower than left ear. Pearson correlations were significant for all tests except two; D-SRT was highly correlated to W-SRT (r = .49-.72) in both ears. Regression analyses for both ears and step sizes revealed that prediction of the hearing threshold for speech from PTA was equally accurate for SRT measured with digit pairs or popular pediatric spondee stimuli. CONCLUSION Digit pairs are an appropriate alternative stimulus for SRT measurement in children with normal hearing. Future diagnostic audiology implications, including application for other pediatric populations, are discussed.


Perceptual and Motor Skills | 2011

Speech intelligibility differences across sound classes with in-the-ear and free-field microphones in quiet.

Julie M. Estis; Julie A. Parisi; Robert E. Moore; Douglas S. Brungart

Speech intelligibility performance with an in-the-ear microphone embedded in a custom-molded deep-insertion earplug was compared with results obtained using a free-field microphone. Intelligibility differences between microphones were further analyzed to assess whether reduced intelligibility was specific to certain sound classes. 36 participants completed the Modified Rhyme Test using recordings made with each microphone. While speech intelligibility for both microphones was highly accurate, intelligibility with the free-field microphone was significantly better than with the in-the-ear microphone. There were significant effects of place and manner of sound production. Significant differences in recognition among specific phonemes were also revealed. Implications included modifying the in-the-ear microphone to transmit more high frequency energy. Use of the in-the-ear microphone was limited by significant loss of high-frequency energy of the speech signal which resulted in reduced intelligibility for some sounds; however, the in-the-ear microphone is a promising technology for effective communication in military environments.


Perceptual and Motor Skills | 2007

Relations of pitch matching, pitch discrimination, and otoacoustic emission suppression in individuals not formally trained as musicians.

Robert E. Moore; Julie M. Estis; Fawen Zhang; Christopher Watts; Elizabeth Marble

Research has yielded a relationship between pitch matching and pitch discrimination. Good pitch matchers tend to be good pitch discriminators and are often judged to be vocally talented. Otoacoustic emission suppression measures the function of the efferent auditory system which may affect accuracy for pitch matching and pitch discrimination. Formally trained musicians show pitch matching and pitch discrimination superior to those of nonmusicians and have greater efferent otoacoustic emission suppression than nonmusicians. This study investigated the relationship among pitch matching, pitch discrimination, and otoacoustic emission suppression in individuals with no formal musical training and who showed varied pitch matching and pitch discrimination. Analysis suggested a significant relationship between pitch matching and pitch discrimination but not between otoacoustic emission suppression and pitch matching and pitch discrimination. Findings are presented in the context of previous research indicating a significant relationship between otoacoustic emission suppression and musical talent in trained musicians.


Journal of Voice | 2006

The Singing Power Ratio as an Objective Measure of Singing Voice Quality in Untrained Talented and Nontalented Singers

Christopher Watts; Kathryn Barnes-Burroughs; Julie M. Estis; Debra J. Blanton


Journal of Voice | 2008

Pitch discrimination and pitch matching abilities with vocal and nonvocal stimuli.

Robert E. Moore; Julie M. Estis; Susan Gordon-Hickey; Christopher Watts

Collaboration


Dive into the Julie M. Estis's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robert E. Moore

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ashley Gaal Flagge

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alison B. Rudd

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ashli Dean-Claytor

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bill Pruitt

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brenda L. Beverly

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Debra J. Blanton

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Douglas S. Brungart

Air Force Research Laboratory

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge