Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Kai Lutz is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Kai Lutz.


Cognitive Brain Research | 2000

Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study

Tony W Buchanan; Kai Lutz; Shahram Mirzazade; Karsten Specht; N. Jon Shah; Karl Zilles; Lutz Jäncke

This study examined the neural areas involved in the recognition of both emotional prosody and phonemic components of words expressed in spoken language using echo-planar, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Ten right-handed males were asked to discriminate words based on either expressed emotional tone (angry, happy, sad, or neutral) or phonemic characteristics, specifically, initial consonant sound (bower, dower, power, or tower). Significant bilateral activity was observed in the detection of both emotional and verbal aspects of language when compared to baseline activity. We found that the detection of emotion compared with verbal detection resulted in significant activity in the right inferior frontal lobe. Conversely, the detection of verbal stimuli compared with the detection of emotion activated left inferior frontal lobe regions most significantly. Specific analysis of the anterior auditory cortex revealed increased right hemisphere activity during the detection of emotion compared to activity during verbal detection. These findings illustrate bilateral involvement in the detection of emotion in language while concomitantly showing significantly lateralized activity in both emotional and verbal detection, in both the temporal and frontal lobes.


Cognitive Brain Research | 2000

Cortical activations during paced finger-tapping applying visual and auditory pacing stimuli.

Lutz Jäncke; R Loose; Kai Lutz; K Specht; N.J. Shah

In order to study neural systems which are involved in motor timing we used whole-brain functional resonance imaging while subjects performed a paced finger-tapping task (PFT) with their right index finger. During one condition, subjects were imaged while tapping in synchrony with tones separated by a constant interval (auditory synchronisation, AS), followed by tapping without the pacing stimulus (auditory continuation, AC). In another condition, subjects were imaged while tapping in synchrony with a visual stimulus presented at the same frequency as the tones (visual synchronisation, VS) followed by a tapping sequence without visual pacing (visual continuation, VC). The following main results were obtained: (1) tapping in the context of visual pacing was generally more variable than tapping in the context of auditory stimuli; (2) during all conditions, a fronto-parietal network was active including the dorsal lateral premotor cortex (dPMC), M1, S1, inferior parietal lobule (LPi), supplementary motor cortex (SMA), the right cerebellar hemisphere, and the paravermial region; (3) stronger activation in the bilateral ventral premotor cortex (vPMC), the left LPi, the SMA, the right inferior cerebellum, and the left thalamus during both auditory conditions (AS and AC) compared to the visual conditions (VS and VC); (4) stronger activation in the right superior cerebellum, the vermis, and the right LPi during the visual conditions (VS and VC); (5) similar activations for the AS and AC conditions; but (6) marked differences between the VS and VC conditions especially in the dorsal premotor cortex (dPMC) and LPi areas; and (7) finally, there were no activations in the auditory and visual cortices when the pacing stimuli were absent. These findings were taken as evidence for a general difference between the motor control modes operative during the auditory and visual conditions. Paced finger tapping in the context of auditory pacing stimuli relies more on brain structures subserving internal motor control while paced finger-tapping in the context of visual pacing stimuli relies on brain structures relying on the subserving processing or imagination of visual pacing stimuli.


Brain Research | 2006

The emotional power of music: How music enhances the feeling of affective pictures

Thomas Baumgartner; Kai Lutz; Conny F. Schmidt; Lutz Jäncke

Music is an intriguing stimulus widely used in movies to increase the emotional experience. However, no brain imaging study has to date examined this enhancement effect using emotional pictures (the modality mostly used in emotion research) and musical excerpts. Therefore, we designed this functional magnetic resonance imaging study to explore how musical stimuli enhance the feeling of affective pictures. In a classical block design carefully controlling for habituation and order effects, we presented fearful and sad pictures (mostly taken from the IAPS) either alone or combined with congruent emotional musical excerpts (classical pieces). Subjective ratings clearly indicated that the emotional experience was markedly increased in the combined relative to the picture condition. Furthermore, using a second-level analysis and regions of interest approach, we observed a clear functional and structural dissociation between the combined and the picture condition. Besides increased activation in brain areas known to be involved in auditory as well as in neutral and emotional visual-auditory integration processes, the combined condition showed increased activation in many structures known to be involved in emotion processing (including for example amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampus, insula, striatum, medial ventral frontal cortex, cerebellum, fusiform gyrus). In contrast, the picture condition only showed an activation increase in the cognitive part of the prefrontal cortex, mainly in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Based on these findings, we suggest that emotional pictures evoke a more cognitive mode of emotion perception, whereas congruent presentations of emotional visual and musical stimuli rather automatically evoke strong emotional feelings and experiences.


Brain Research | 2007

A network for audio-motor coordination in skilled pianists and non-musicians

Simon Baumann; Susan Koeneke; Conny F. Schmidt; Martin Meyer; Kai Lutz; Lutz Jäncke

Playing a musical instrument requires efficient auditory and motor processing. Fast feed forward and feedback connections that link the acoustic target to the corresponding motor programs need to be established during years of practice. The aim of our study is to provide a detailed description of cortical structures that participate in this audio-motor coordination network in professional pianists and non-musicians. In order to map these interacting areas using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we considered cortical areas that are concurrently activated during silent piano performance and motionless listening to piano sound. Furthermore we investigated to what extent interactions between the auditory and the motor modality happen involuntarily. We observed a network of predominantly secondary and higher order areas belonging to the auditory and motor modality. The extent of activity was clearly increased by imagination of the absent modality. However, this network did neither comprise primary auditory nor primary motor areas in any condition. Activity in the lateral dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) and the pre-supplementary motor cortex (preSMA) was significantly increased for pianists. Our data imply an intermodal transformation network of auditory and motor areas which is subject to a certain degree of plasticity by means of intensive training.


Brain and Language | 2001

Focused and Nonfocused Attention in Verbal and Emotional Dichotic Listening: An FMRI Study

Lutz Jäncke; T.W. Buchanan; Kai Lutz; N.J. Shah

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to identify cortical regions which are involved in two dichotic listening tasks. During one task the subjects were required to allocate attention to both ears and to detect a specific target word (phonetic task), while during a second task the subjects were required to detect a specific emotional tone (emotional task). During three attentional conditions of each task, the subjects were required to focus attention to the right (FR) or left ear (FL), while during a third condition subjects were required to allocate attention to both ears simultaneously. In 11 right-handed male subjects, these dichotic listening tasks evoked strong activations in a temporofrontal network involving auditory cortices located in the temporal lobe and prefrontal brain regions. Hemodynamic responses were measured in the following regions of interest: Heschls gyrus (HG), the planum polare (PP), the planum temporale (PT), the anterior superior temporal sulcus (aSTS), the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), and the inferior frontal gyrus region (IFG) of both hemispheres. The following findings were obtained: (1) the degree of activation in HG and PP depends on the direction of attention. In particular it was found that selectively attending to right-ear input led to increased activity specifically in the left HG and PP and attention to left ear input increased right-sided activity in these structures; (2) hemodynamic responses in the PT, aSTS, pSTS, and IFG were not modulated by the different focused-attention conditions; (3) hemodynamic responses in HG and PP in the nonforced conditions were the sum activation of the forced conditions; (4) there was no general difference between the phonetic and emotion tasks in terms of hemodynamic responses; (5) hemodynamic responses in the PT and pSTS were strongly left-lateralized, reflecting the specialization of these brain regions for language processing. These findings are discussed in the context of current theories of hemispheric specialization.


Neuron | 2009

The neural circuitry of a broken promise

Thomas Baumgartner; Urs Fischbacher; Anja Feierabend; Kai Lutz; Ernst Fehr

Promises are one of the oldest human-specific psychological mechanisms fostering cooperation and trust. Here, we study the neural underpinnings of promise keeping and promise breaking. Subjects first make a promise decision (promise stage), then they anticipate whether the promise affects the interaction partners decision (anticipation stage) and are subsequently free to keep or break the promise (decision stage). Findings revealed that the breaking of the promise is associated with increased activation in the DLPFC, ACC, and amygdala, suggesting that the dishonest act involves an emotional conflict due to the suppression of the honest response. Moreover, the breach of the promise can be predicted by a perfidious brain activity pattern (anterior insula, ACC, inferior frontal gyrus) during the promise and anticipation stage, indicating that brain measurements may reveal malevolent intentions before dishonest or deceitful acts are actually committed.


Neuroreport | 2000

Tapping movements according to regular and irregular visual timing signals investigated with fMRI.

Kai Lutz; Karsten Specht; N.J. Shah; Lutz Jäncke

Whole-head functional MR images were acquired while 10 subjects were asked to tap with their right index finger in synchrony with a visual stimulus appearing regularly with a frequency of 1.5 Hz, or irregularly with a mean frequency of 1.5 Hz. Performance data show that during regular tapping most taps were close to stimulus onset. However, when the subjects paced their tapping according to the irregular stimuli, most taps appeared about 300 ms after the onset of the pacing stimuli. Comparing the brain activations resulting from regular tapping with those from irregular tapping, we found increased activation in left precuneus only. Comparing irregular versus regular tapping shows increased activity in right cerebellar nuclei and vermis, left ventrolateral thalamus, left sensorimotor cortex, left and right pre-SMA and left SMA proper. These results show that during irregular pacing the motor areas are more strongly activated than during regular pacing. In addition, further neural systems are involved in the motor control during irregular pacing: cerebellar vermis and a cerebellothalamo-cortical system. The latter is supposedly involved in error correction in the context of visually guided movements.


NeuroImage | 2005

Scanning silence: Mental imagery of complex sounds

Nico Bunzeck; Torsten Wuestenberg; Kai Lutz; Hans-Jochen Heinze; Lutz Jäncke

In this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, we investigated the neural basis of mental auditory imagery of familiar complex sounds that did not contain language or music. In the first condition (perception), the subjects watched familiar scenes and listened to the corresponding sounds that were presented simultaneously. In the second condition (imagery), the same scenes were presented silently and the subjects had to mentally imagine the appropriate sounds. During the third condition (control), the participants watched a scrambled version of the scenes without sound. To overcome the disadvantages of the stray acoustic scanner noise in auditory fMRI experiments, we applied sparse temporal sampling technique with five functional clusters that were acquired at the end of each movie presentation. Compared to the control condition, we found bilateral activations in the primary and secondary auditory cortices (including Heschls gyrus and planum temporale) during perception of complex sounds. In contrast, the imagery condition elicited bilateral hemodynamic responses only in the secondary auditory cortex (including the planum temporale). No significant activity was observed in the primary auditory cortex. The results show that imagery and perception of complex sounds that do not contain language or music rely on overlapping neural correlates of the secondary but not primary auditory cortex.


Neuroscience Letters | 1998

A parametric analysis of the 'rate effect' in the sensorimotor cortex : a functional magnetic resonance imaging analysis in human subjects

Lutz Jäncke; Karsten Specht; Shahram Mirzazade; Rainer Loose; Marc Himmelbach; Kai Lutz; Nadim Joni Shah

We studied the effects of different movement speeds of unimanual right hand movements on functional magnetic resonance signal changes in the sensorimotor cortex using echo planar imaging (EPI). Six healthy right-handed subjects were scanned at rest and while executing a finger tapping task with their right index finger. Movement frequency was visually paced at rates ranging from 0.5 to 5 Hz, separated by 0.5 Hz steps. The blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response within the left sensorimotor cortex was linearly and positively related to movement frequency. However, this relation holds (r2 = 0.91) only for movement frequencies faster than 1 Hz (1.5-5 Hz). For the slower frequencies there was an initial sharp increase of the BOLD response from 0.5 to 1 Hz followed by an activity drop for 1.5 Hz. These results are compatible with the idea that two different motor control modes are operative during slow or fast movements. During slow movements a computational demanding on-line feedback control mode is operative resulting in strong BOLD signals indicating extensive neural activity. During faster movements on the other hand a program-like motor control mode is operative resulting in less demanding neural computations. The amount of neural computation for the latter control mode increases with increasing movement speed.


Experimental Brain Research | 2006

Extensive training of elementary finger tapping movements changes the pattern of motor cortex excitability

Susan Koeneke; Kai Lutz; Uwe Herwig; Ulf Ziemann; Lutz Jäncke

There is evidence of a strong capacity for functional and structural reorganization in the human motor system. However, past research has focused mainly on complex movement sequences over rather short training durations. In this study we investigated changes in corticospinal excitability associated with longer training of elementary, maximum-speed tapping movements. All participating subjects were consistent right-handers and were trained using either the right (experiment 1) or the left thumb (experiment 2). Transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied to obtain motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from the abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle of the right and the left hand before and after training. As a result of training, a significant increase was observed in tapping speed accompanied by increased MEPs, recorded from the trained APB muscle, following contralateral M1 stimulation. In the case of subdominant-hand training we additionally demonstrate increased MEP amplitudes evoked at the right APB (untrained hand) in the first training week. Enhanced corticospinal excitability associated with practice of elementary movements may constitute a necessary precursor for inducing plastic changes within the motor system. The involvement of the ipsilateral left M1 likely reflects the predominant role of the left M1 in the general control (modification) of simple motor parameters in right-handed subjects.

Collaboration


Dive into the Kai Lutz's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

N.J. Shah

Forschungszentrum Jülich

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hans-Jochen Heinze

Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge