Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Kang Lee is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Kang Lee.


Psychological Science | 2014

Can Classic Moral Stories Promote Honesty in Children

Kang Lee; Victoria Talwar; Anjanie McCarthy; Ilana Ross; Angela D. Evans; Cindy Arruda

The classic moral stories have been used extensively to teach children about the consequences of lying and the virtue of honesty. Despite their widespread use, there is no evidence whether these stories actually promote honesty in children. This study compared the effectiveness of four classic moral stories in promoting honesty in 3- to 7-year-olds. Surprisingly, the stories of “Pinocchio” and “The Boy Who Cried Wolf” failed to reduce lying in children. In contrast, the apocryphal story of “George Washington and the Cherry Tree” significantly increased truth telling. Further results suggest that the reason for the difference in honesty-promoting effectiveness between the “George Washington” story and the other stories was that the former emphasizes the positive consequences of honesty, whereas the latter focus on the negative consequences of dishonesty. When the “George Washington” story was altered to focus on the negative consequences of dishonesty, it too failed to promote honesty in children.


NeuroImage | 2014

Neural correlates of second-order verbal deception: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study.

Xiao Pan Ding; Liyang Sai; Genyue Fu; Jiangang Liu; Kang Lee

The present study focused on neural correlates underlying second-order deception. In first-order deception, the recipient of deception is unaware of the deceivers deceptive intention. However, during second-order deception, the recipient is fully aware of the deceivers deceptive intention and thus the deceiver needs to use both lies and truths to deceive the recipient. Using the functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) methodology and a naturalistic interactive game, we found that second-order deception elicited significantly greater [oxy-Hb] changes in the prefrontal cortex (the right superior frontal gyrus (SFG), BA6) than the non-deceptive control condition. This finding suggests that second-order deception, like first-order deception, engages specifically the cortical regions associated with the planning of complex actions and goal processing. We also found that lying to deceive produced greater neural activities in the right middle frontal gyrus than truth-telling to deceive. This suggests that although both actions serve deceptive purposes, making a false statement contradicting the true state of affairs still requires more executive control and thus greater neural responses in the cortical regions associated with this function. In addition, we found that the successful deception produced greater neural activities in a broad area of the prefrontal frontal cortex than failure to deceive, indicating the involvement of the cortical reward system during second-order deception. Further, failure of truth-telling to deceive produced greater neural responses in the right SFG than failure of lying to deceive. The present findings taken together suggest that second-order deception engages both the cortical executive and reward systems.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2014

Elementary school children's cheating behavior and its cognitive correlates

Xiao Pan Ding; Danielle S. Omrin; Angela D. Evans; Genyue Fu; Guopeng Chen; Kang Lee

Elementary school childrens cheating behavior and its cognitive correlates were investigated using a guessing game. Children (n=95) between 8 and 12 years of age were asked to guess which side of the screen a coin would appear on and received rewards based on their self-reported accuracy. Childrens cheating behavior was measured by examining whether children failed to adhere to the game rules by falsely reporting their accuracy. Childrens theory-of-mind understanding and executive functioning skills were also assessed. The majority of children cheated during the guessing game, and cheating behavior decreased with age. Children with better working memory and inhibitory control were less likely to cheat. However, among the cheaters, those with greater cognitive flexibility use more tactics while cheating. Results revealed the unique role that executive functioning plays in childrens cheating behavior: Like a double-edged sword, executive functioning can inhibit childrens cheating behavior, on the one hand, while it can promote the sophistication of childrens cheating tactics, on the other.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2012

Cultural differences in the development of cognitive shifting: East-West comparison

Yusuke Moriguchi; Angela D. Evans; Kazuo Hiraki; Shoji Itakura; Kang Lee

Prior research has documented that Japanese childrens performance on the Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) task can be influenced by their observation of another person completing the task, which is referred to as social transmission of disinhibition. The current study explored whether Canadian children would also show a social transmission of disinhibition and whether their performance would be comparable to that of Japanese children. In this study, 3- and 4-year-olds in Canada and Japan were given both the standard version and social version of the DCCS. Results indicated that Canadian children displayed the social transmission of disinhibition, but their effects were significantly weaker than those with Japanese children. On the other hand, performance on the standard DCCS was comparable between children in the two countries. We discuss the results in terms of cultural differences in the relationship between self and other.


Child Development | 2016

Implicit Racial Biases in Preschool Children and Adults from Asia and Africa.

Miao K. Qian; Gail D. Heyman; Paul C. Quinn; Francoise A. Messi; Genyue Fu; Kang Lee

This research used an Implicit Racial Bias Test to investigate implicit racial biases among 3- to 5-year-olds and adult participants in China (N = 213) and Cameroon (N = 257). In both cultures, participants displayed high levels of racial biases that remained stable between 3 and 5 years of age. Unlike adults, young childrens implicit racial biases were unaffected by the social status of the other-race groups. Also, unlike adults, young children displayed overt explicit racial biases, and these biases were dissociated from their implicit biases. The results provide strong evidence for the early emergence of implicit racial biases and point to the need to reduce them in early childhood.


Developmental Science | 2016

Young children with a positive reputation to maintain are less likely to cheat.

Genyue Fu; Gail D. Heyman; Miao Qian; Tengfei Guo; Kang Lee

The present study examined whether having a positive reputation to maintain makes young children less likely to cheat. Cheating was assessed through a temptation resistance paradigm in which participants were instructed not to cheat in a guessing game. Across three studies (total N = 361), preschool-aged participants were randomly assigned to either a reputation condition, in which an experimenter told them that she had learned of their positive reputation from classmates, or to a control condition in which they received no such information. By age 5, children in the reputation condition cheated less often than those in the control condition even though nobody was watching and choosing not to cheat conflicted with their personal interest. These findings are the first to show that informing children that they have a positive reputation to maintain can influence their moral behavior.


Child Development | 2014

Social and Cognitive Factors Associated With Children's Secret-Keeping for a Parent

Heidi M. Gordon; Thomas D. Lyon; Kang Lee

This study examined childrens secret-keeping for a parent and its relation to trust, theory of mind, secrecy endorsement, and executive functioning (EF). Children (Nxa0=xa0107) between 4 and 12xa0years of age participated in a procedure wherein parents broke a toy and asked children to promise secrecy. Responses to open-ended and direct questions were examined. Overall, secret-keeping increased with age and promising to keep the secret was related to fewer disclosures in open-ended questioning. Children who kept the secret in direct questioning exhibited greater trust and better parental ratings of EF than children who disclosed the secret. Findings highlight the importance of both social and cognitive factors in secret-keeping development.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2015

Children trust people who lie to benefit others.

Genyue Fu; Gail D. Heyman; Guowei Chen; Peilong Liu; Kang Lee

The current research examined whether children consider who benefits from lies when judging the trustworthiness of liars. Across two studies (total N=214), 6- to 11-year-olds trusted individuals who lied to promote the interests of others, but not those who lied to promote their own interests. In contrast, children trusted individuals who told the truth regardless of who benefited. Trust in individuals who lied to promote the interests of others was evident even in the absence of moral approval for their actions. These results demonstrate that children take into account both the truth value of a speakers statements and who benefits when assessing trustworthiness and that moral approval is not a prerequisite for trust.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2015

Eliciting promises from children reduces cheating

Gail D. Heyman; Genyue Fu; Jianyan Lin; Miao K. Qian; Kang Lee

Widespread cheating can undermine rules that are necessary for maintaining social order. Preventing cheating can be a challenge, especially with regard to children, who as a result of their limited executive function skills may have particular difficulty with resisting temptation to cheat. We examined one approach designed to help children resist this temptation: eliciting a verbal commitment to not cheat. We tested 4- to 7-year-olds (total N = 330) and found that starting at 5 years of age, a verbal commitment to not cheat led to a substantial reduction in cheating. The results suggest that verbal commitments can be used to help children overcome temptations and comply with rules.


Developmental Psychology | 2014

Japanese and American Children's Moral Evaluations of Reporting on Transgressions

Ivy Chiu Loke; Gail D. Heyman; Shoji Itakura; Rie Toriyama; Kang Lee

American and Japanese childrens evaluations of the reporting of peers transgressions to authority figures were investigated. Seven-, 9-, and 11-year-old children (N = 160) and adults (N = 62) were presented with vignettes and were asked to evaluate the decisions of child observers who reported their friends either major or relatively minor transgression to a teacher. The results showed that, in both countries, participants across all age groups considered it appropriate to report major transgressions. However, compared with older participants, the youngest children thought it was appropriate to tattle (i.e., to report more minor transgressions). The results also showed a cross-cultural difference: Japanese compared with American participants considered it appropriate to report minor transgressions. The age-related findings are discussed with reference to childrens social experience and improving cognition. The cross-cultural findings are discussed with reference to potential differences in the emphases placed on respecting authority relationships, empathy, and social interdependence by the 2 cultures.

Collaboration


Dive into the Kang Lee's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gail D. Heyman

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Genyue Fu

Hangzhou Normal University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Xiao Pan Ding

Zhejiang Normal University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Miao K. Qian

Hangzhou Normal University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Thomas D. Lyon

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Biyun Chen

Zhejiang Sci-Tech University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge