Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Keith McBurnett is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Keith McBurnett.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology | 1994

Psychopathy and conduct problems in children.

Paul J. Frick; Bridget S. O'Brien; Jane M. Wootton; Keith McBurnett

Factor analysis of a measure of psychopathy was conducted in a sample of 95 clinic-referred children between the ages of 6 and 13 years. These analyses revealed 2 dimensions of behavior, one associated with impulsivity and conduct problems (I/CP) and one associated with the interpersonal and motivational aspects of psychopathy (callous/unemotional: CU). In a subset of this sample (n = 64), analyses indicated that scores on the I/CP factor were highly associated with traditional measures of conduct problems. In contrast, scores derived from the CU factor were only moderately associated with measures of conduct problems and exhibited a different pattern of associations on several criteria that have been associated with psychopathy (e.g., sensation seeking) or childhood antisocial behavior (e.g., low intelligence, poor school achievement, and anxiety). These analyses suggest that psychopathic personality features and conduct problems are independent, yet interacting, constructs in children, analogous to findings in the adult literature.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology | 2012

Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes.

Erik G. Willcutt; Joel T. Nigg; Bruce F. Pennington; Mary V. Solanto; Luis Augusto Rohde; Rosemary Tannock; Sandra K. Loo; Caryn L. Carlson; Keith McBurnett; Benjamin B. Lahey

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV) criteria for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) specify two dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms that are used to define three nominal subtypes: predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type (ADHD-H), predominantly inattentive type (ADHD-I), and combined type (ADHD-C). To aid decision making for DSM-5 and other future diagnostic systems, a comprehensive literature review and meta-analysis of 546 studies was completed to evaluate the validity of the DSM-IV model of ADHD. Results indicated that DSM-IV criteria identify individuals with significant and persistent impairment in social, academic, occupational, and adaptive functioning when intelligence, demographic factors, and concurrent psychopathology are controlled. Available data overwhelmingly support the concurrent, predictive, and discriminant validity of the distinction between inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms, and indicate that nearly all differences among the nominal subtypes are consistent with the relative levels of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms that define the subtypes. In contrast, the DSM-IV subtype model is compromised by weak evidence for the validity of ADHD-H after first grade, minimal support for the distinction between ADHD-I and ADHD-C in studies of etiological influences, academic and cognitive functioning, and treatment response, and the marked longitudinal instability of all three subtypes. Overall, we conclude that the DSM-IV ADHD subtypes provide a convenient clinical shorthand to describe the functional and behavioral correlates of current levels of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms, but do not identify discrete subgroups with sufficient long-term stability to justify the classification of distinct forms of the disorder. Empirical support is stronger for an alternative model that would replace the subtypes with dimensional modifiers that reflect the number of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms at the time of assessment. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved).


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 1999

Annotation: the development of antisocial behavior: an integrative causal model.

Benjamin B. Lahey; Irwin D. Waldman; Keith McBurnett

In this paper we have described an integrative causal model of the development of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. The present model primarily integrates several previous models, but offers some new testable hypotheses. We believe that stable individual differences in propensity to antisocial behavior reflect variations in a number of dimensions of predisposing temperament and cognitive ability, each with its own genetic and environmental influences. The dimensions of predisposing temperament include oppositionality, harm avoidance, and callousness. Genetic influences are predicted to have only indirect effects on antisocial behavior via their influence on predisposition and on the youths social environment. Environmental influences are expected to be important contributors to antisocial propensity, but these environmental influences reflect, in part, the genetic influences on the dimensions of predisposition (i.e. genotype-environment covariance). We also hypothesize that the levels of influence of the factors that determine individual differences in antisocial propensity change with development, such that genetic influences are of greater magnitude in early childhood and social influences contribute more strongly during later childhood and adolescence (both through independent effects and genotype-environment covariance). However, low levels of heritable predisposing child characteristics may protect against peer influences at all ages.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 1988

Psychopathology in the Parents of Children with Conduct Disorder and Hyperactivity

Benjamin B. Lahey; John Piacentini; Keith McBurnett; Penny Stone; Sandra Hartdaghn; George W. Hynd

Abstract The biological parents of 86 outpatient children aged 6 to 1 3 years were assessed using a structured diagnostic interview. Both mothers and fathers of children with Conduct Disorder (CD) (N = 37) were more likely to exhibit Antisocial Personality Disorder, and fathers were more likely to abuse substances. CD mothers exhibited more depression and the triad of Antisocial Personality Disorder. Substance Abuse, or Somatization Disorder. In contrast. Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity (ADD/H) (N = 18) was not associated with any parental disorder. However, fathers of children with both CD and ADD/H, which is associated with greater aggression and persistent law-breaking in children than CD alone, were markedly more likely to have a history of aggression, arrest, and imprisonment. These results confirm findings that CD is linked to parental psychopathology but ADD/ H is not. When CD and ADD/H co-occur, however, there is markedly more aggression and illegal activity in both fathers and children. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc Psychiatry , 1988, 27. 2:163–170.


Exceptional Children | 1993

Effect of Stimulant Medication on Children with Attention Deficit Disorder: A “Review of Reviews”

James M. Swanson; Keith McBurnett; Tim Wigal; Linda J. Pfiffner; Marc Lerner; Lillie Williams; Diane L. Christian; Leanne Tamm; Erik G. Willcutt; Kent Crowley; Walter Clevenger; Nader Khouzam; Christina Woo; Francis M. Crinella; Todd D. Fisher

The University of California, Irvine ADD Center recently conducted a synthesis of the literature on the use of stimulants with children with attention deficit disorder (ADD), using a unique “review of reviews” methodology. In this article, we compare three reviews from each of three review types (traditional, meta-analytic, general audience) and illustrate how coding variables can highlight sources of divergence. In general, divergent conclusions stemmed from variations in goal rather than from variations in the sources selected to review. Across quantitative reviews, the average effect size for symptomatic improvement (.83) was twice that for benefits on IQ and achievement measures (.35). A summary of what should and should not be expected of the use of stimulants with ADD children, derived from the literature synthesis, is provided.


Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology | 2001

Symptom properties as a function of ADHD type: an argument for continued study of sluggish cognitive tempo.

Keith McBurnett; Linda J. Pfiffner; Paul J. Frick

Inconsistent alertness and orientation (sluggishness, drowsiness, daydreaming) were reported to accompany Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) without Hyperactivity in DSM-III. Such Sluggish Cognitive Tempo items were tested in the DSM-IV Field Trial for ADHD, but were discarded from the Inattention symptom list because of poor negative predictive power. Using 692 children referred to a pediatric subspecialty clinic for ADHD, Sluggish Tempo items were re-evaluated. When Hyperactivity–Impulsivity was absent (i.e., using only cases of Inattentive Type plus clinic controls), Sluggish Tempo items showed substantially improved utility as symptoms of Inattention. Factor analyses distinguished a Sluggish Tempo factor from an Inattention factor. When DSM-IV ADHD types were compared, Inattentive Type was uniquely elevated on Sluggish Tempo. These findings suggest that (a) Sluggish Tempo items are adequate symptoms for Inattentive Type, or (b) Sluggish Tempo may distinguish two subtypes of Inattentive Type. Either conclusion is incompatible with ADHD nosology in DSM-IV.


Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology | 1997

Social skills training with parent generalization: Treatment effects for children with attention deficit disorder.

Linda J. Pfiffner; Keith McBurnett

The effectiveness of brief social skills training (SST) was evaluated in a controlled outcome study with 27 children meeting criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., revised; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) for an attention deficit disorder. Children were randomly assigned to either SST with parent-mediated generalization (SST-PG), child-only SST, or a wait-list control group. SST consisted of 8 group sessions in which skill modules were taught sequentially. Parents of children in the SST-PG group simultaneously participated in group generalization training designed to support their childrens transfer of skills. Significant improvement in childrens skill knowledge and in parent reports of social skills and disruptive behavior occurred for both treatment groups relative to the wait-list control group and maintained at a 4-month follow-up. More modest evidence was found for generalization of SST to the school setting.


Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology | 2000

Behavioral versus behavioral and pharmacological treatment in ADHD children attending a summer treatment program.

E William PelhamJr.; Elizabeth M. Gnagy; Andrew R. Greiner; Betsy Hoza; Stephen P. Hinshaw; James M. Swanson; Steve Simpson; Cheri Shapiro; Oscar G. Bukstein; Carrie Baron-Myak; Keith McBurnett

As part of the behavioral treatment in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA), children participated in an intensive summer treatment program (STP). This study examined the differences between 57 children in the combined treatment (Comb) group, who were medicated, and 60 children in the behavioral treatment (Beh) group, who were unmedicated throughout the STP. Comb children were significantly better than Beh on 5 measures: rule following, good sportsmanship, peer negative nominations, and STP teacher posttreatment ratings of inattention/overactivity. Groups did not differ on any of the other 30 measures, and responded similarly to the STP over time. Comparisons to normative data revealed that Comb children were more likely to fall within the normative range on 6 measures. The differences between these results and the main MTA results, in which Comb was always superior to Beh, are discussed in terms of the relative intensity of combined treatments. The implications for future studies of pharmacological and behavioral treatment for ADHD are discussed.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 1991

Anxiety, Inhibition, and Conduct Disorder in Children: I. Relations to Social Impairment

Jason L. Walker; Benjamin B. Lahey; Mary F. Russo; Paul J. Frick; Mary Anne G. Christ; Keith McBurnett; Rolf Loeber; Magda Stouthamer-Loeber; Stephanie M. Green

Jeffrey A. Gray has proposed a model in which conduct disorder (CD) is viewed as the result of both excessive activity of a behavioral activation system that mediates appetitive and aggressive behavior and deficient activity of a behavioral inhibition system that mediates both anxiety and the inhibition of behavior in the presence of cues signalling impending punishment or frustration. The relation of anxiety to antisocial behavior was examined in 177 clinic-referred boys, aged 7 to 12 years, 68 of whom met DSM-III-R criteria for CD. As predicted by Grays model, boys with CD and comorbid anxiety disorder were markedly less impaired than boys with CD alone.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 1998

Validity of DSM-IV subtypes of conduct disorder based on age of onset

Benjamin B. Lahey; Rolf Loeber; Herbert C. Quay; Brooks Applegate; David Shaffer; Irwin D. Waldman; Elizabeth L. Hart; Keith McBurnett; Paul J. Frick; Peter S. Jensen; Mina K. Dulcan; Glorisa Canino; Hector R. Bird

OBJECTIVE To present data from the DSM-IV field trials that led to the distinction between subtypes of conduct disorder (CD) that emerge in childhood or adolescence. In addition, data from a household sample were used to attempt to cross-validate these findings. METHOD Differences between youths who met criteria for the two subtypes of CD were examined in the field trials sample of 440 youths aged 4 through 17 years and in a household sample of 1,285 youths aged 9 through 17 years. RESULTS In both samples, there was a steep decline in aggression occurring around an age of onset of 10 years, but the number of nonaggressive behaviors was unrelated to the age of onset of CD. In the field trials sample, youths who met criteria for the adolescent-onset type were more likely to be girls, less likely to meet criteria for oppositional defiant disorder, and less likely to have a family history of antisocial behavior than the childhood-onset type, but these latter findings were not confirmed in the household sample. CONCLUSIONS The DSM-IV approach to subtyping CD distinguishes subgroups that differ markedly in level of physical aggression. The advantages of a developmental approach to subtyping are discussed.

Collaboration


Dive into the Keith McBurnett's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rolf Loeber

University of Pittsburgh

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Paul J. Frick

Australian Catholic University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Miguel T. Villodas

Florida International University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mary Rooney

University of California

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge