Kelly A. Gildersleeve
University of California, Los Angeles
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Kelly A. Gildersleeve.
Current Directions in Psychological Science | 2011
Martie G. Haselton; Kelly A. Gildersleeve
In contrast to our closest cousin, the chimpanzee, humans appear at first to lack cues of impending ovulation that would mark the fertile period in which a female can become pregnant. Consequently, that ovulation is “concealed” in women has long been the consensus among scientists studying human mating. A recent series of studies shows, however, that there are discernible cues of fertility in women’s social behaviors, body scents, voices, and, possibly, aspects of physical beauty. Some of these changes are subtle, but others are strikingly large (we report effect sizes ranging from small, d = 0.12 to large, d = 1.20). Moreover, emerging evidence suggests that women’s male partners may adaptively shift their behavior in response to cues of approaching ovulation. These results have far-reaching implications for understanding fluctuations in attraction, conflict, and relationship dynamics.
Hormones and Behavior | 2012
Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Martie G. Haselton; Christina M. Larson; Elizabeth G. Pillsworth
Scent communication plays a central role in the mating behavior of many nonhuman mammals but has often been overlooked in the study of human mating. However, a growing body of evidence suggests that men may perceive womens high-fertility body scents (collected near ovulation) as more attractive than their low-fertility body scents. The present study provides a methodologically rigorous replication of this finding, while also examining several novel questions. Women collected samples of their natural body scent twice--once on a low-fertility day and once on a high-fertility day of the ovulatory cycle. Tests of luteinizing hormone confirmed that women experienced ovulation within two days of their high-fertility session. Men smelled each womans high- and low-fertility scent samples and completed discrimination and preference tasks. At above-chance levels, men accurately discriminated between womens high- and low-fertility scent samples (61%) and chose womens high-fertility scent samples as more attractive than their low-fertility scent samples (56%). Men also rated each scent sample on sexiness, pleasantness, and intensity, and estimated the physical attractiveness of the woman who had provided the sample. Multilevel modeling revealed that, when high- and low-fertility scent samples were easier to discriminate from each other, high-fertility scent samples received even more favorable ratings compared with low-fertility scent samples. This study builds on a growing body of evidence indicating that men are attracted to cues of impending ovulation in women and raises the intriguing question of whether womens cycling hormones influence mens attraction and sexual approach behavior.
Hormones and Behavior | 2013
Christina M. Larson; Martie G. Haselton; Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Elizabeth G. Pillsworth
According to the dual mating hypothesis, women possess two overlapping suites of mate-choice mechanisms: one leading to preferences for sexually desirable men who have high-fitness genes and one leading to preferences for men who are able to invest in a woman and her children. Evidence increasingly demonstrates that womens preference for sexual desirability (but not investment attractiveness) increases when women are most fertile within the ovulatory cycle. Little is known, however, about the implications of these preference shifts for womens relationships with their long-term partners. Using luteinizing hormone tests to verify ovulation, across two studies (Samples 1 and 2), we found that women whose partners were relatively low in sexual desirability felt less close to their partner (Samples 1 and 2) and were more critical of their partners faults (Sample 2) on high-fertility days of the cycle just prior to ovulation compared with low-fertility days of the cycle. Women whose partners were relatively high in sexual desirability felt closer to their partner (Sample 1) and more satisfied with their relationship (Sample 2) on high- than low-fertility days of the cycle. There were no such shifts in womens commitment to their relationship. Therefore, partner sexual desirability predicts womens high-fertility assessments of relationship quality but not their intentions to stay in their relationship, consistent with the dual mating hypothesis. These findings suggest that variations across the ovulation cycle in womens reproductive hormones play an important role in relationship dynamics.
Hormones and Behavior | 2014
Melissa R. Fales; Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Martie G. Haselton
The challenge hypothesis posits that male testosterone levels increase in the presence of fertile females to facilitate mating and increase further in the presence of male rivals to facilitate male-male competition. This hypothesis has been supported in a number of nonhuman animal species. We conducted an experiment to test the challenge hypothesis in men. Thirty-four men were randomly assigned to view high-competitive or low-competitive male rivals at high and low fertility within their partners ovulatory cycle (confirmed by luteinizing hormone tests). Testosterone was measured upon arrival to the lab and before and after the manipulation. Based on the challenge hypothesis, we predicted that a) mens baseline testosterone would be higher at high relative to low fertility within their partners cycle, and b) mens testosterone would be higher in response to high-competitive rivals, but not in response to low-competitive rivals, at high relative to low fertility within their partners cycle. Contrary to the first prediction, mens baseline testosterone levels did not differ across high and low fertility. However, consistent with the second prediction, men exposed to high-competitive rivals showed significantly higher post-test testosterone levels at high relative to low fertility, controlling for pre-test testosterone levels. Men exposed to low-competitive rivals showed no such pattern (though the fertility by competition condition interaction fell short of statistical significance). This preliminary support for the challenge hypothesis in men builds on a growing empirical literature suggesting that men possess mating adaptations sensitive to fertility cues emitted by their female partners.
Psychological Bulletin | 2014
Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Martie G. Haselton; Melissa R. Fales
Evolution and Human Behavior | 2016
Steven W. Gangestad; Martie G. Haselton; Lisa L. M. Welling; Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Elizabeth G. Pillsworth; Robert P. Burriss; Christina M. Larson; David A. Puts
Psychological Bulletin | 2014
Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Martie G. Haselton; Melissa R. Fales
Evolution and Human Behavior | 2014
Aaron W. Lukaszewski; Christina M. Larson; Kelly A. Gildersleeve; James R. Roney; Martie G. Haselton
Sex Roles | 2013
Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Lisa M. DeBruine; Martie G. Haselton; David A. Frederick; Ian S. Penton-Voak; Benedict C. Jones; David I. Perrett
Personality and Individual Differences | 2016
Melissa R. Fales; David A. Frederick; Justin R. Garcia; Kelly A. Gildersleeve; Martie G. Haselton; Helen E. Fisher