Kenneth B. Melvin
University of Alabama
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Kenneth B. Melvin.
Criminal Justice and Behavior | 1985
Kenneth B. Melvin; Lorraine K. Gramling; William M. Gardner
The construction and validation of the Attitudes toward Prisoners (ATP) scale are described. Factor analysis resulted in a 36-item Likert scale with the items correlating at least .47 with a general factor. The ATP scale possesses moderate to high split-half (r= .84 to .92) and test-retest (r= .82) reliability. No evidence of response distortion was found. A number of comparisons between selected groups provided considerable evidence of validity. Groups of prisoners, as well as persons engaged in prisoner rehabilitation or prison reform, scored highest on the scale. Intermediate-scoring groups consisted of students, a community sample, and correctional officers. As predicted, the group with lowest average ATP scores consisted of law enforcement officers.
Teaching of Psychology | 1988
Kenneth B. Melvin
To reduce the subjectivity of class participation grades, a method was devised that combined forced-distribution peer ratings with professor grades. In seven seminar courses, correlations between professor and peer ratings ranged from .83 to .90. Course/teacher evaluations were high and the prof/peer technique was generally perceived as a fair way to evaluate participation.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1988
William M. Gardner; Kenneth B. Melvin
A “study guide technique” that successfully detected cheating in college students is described. Also, details of the construction of an attitude toward cheating (ATC) scale are presented. The ATC scale was found to have adequate reliability. Preliminary evidence of scale validity was provided by (1) a correlation of −.30 between ATC scores and cheating on the study guide and (2) a more negative attitude toward cheating (as measured by the ATC scale) exhibited by faculty than by students.
Psychonomic science | 1969
Kenneth B. Melvin; John E. Anson
Male Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) were reinforced for swimming through an aperture by a mirror presentation which released an innate aggressive display. Moderately intense shock-punishment of the operant response increased the vigor of the display and, temporarily, the rates of the operant response. Gradual decreases in rate within and across nonpunished sessions reflected short-term and long-term habituation to the mirror image.
Psychological Reports | 1965
Kenneth B. Melvin; George I. Athey; Frederick H. Heasley
Rats were given shock-escape training in a 4-ft. runway, then divided into 4 groups of 9 Ss each. During extinction, shock was present in the 4-ft. alley, the first 1-ft. segment, or the last 1-ft. segment for 3 groups, but was never present in the start box. A control group received no shock. In general, punished Ss ran faster and longer than non-shocked Ss. The more immediate the punishment, the more vigorous and sustained was the punished act. The immediate 1-ft. shock led to very high resistance to extinction, indicating that an intermediate shock duration may yield optimal facilitation.
Psychonomic science | 1964
R. Chris Martin; Kenneth B. Melvin
Rats were given shock-escape training in a 4 ft runway, then divided into 3 groups of 10 Ss each. During extinction one group received 1 ft of shock immediately after leaving the start box, a second group was shocked in the last 1 ft section of the alley, and a control group was never shocked. The immediate-shock group was found to be more resistant to extinction, followed successively by the delayed-shock group and the no-shock group.
Animal Behaviour | 1969
Kenneth B. Melvin; F. Thomas Cloar
Abstract Following training on a variable interval 2-min reinforcement schedule, bobwhite quail were shown a hawk or a pigeon twice daily for 4 days, then shifted to presentations of the other stimulus for 3 days. Ten-sec presentations of the perched hawk initially released a strong freezing response, thus suppressing key-pecking. The pigeon presentations led to significantly less suppression, but tended to reduce the effect of initial hawk presentations. Habituation to the live hawk was rapid; no recovery of the freezing response was observed. These results point out the modifiability of this type of innate behaviour.
Psychological Record | 1970
Kenneth B. Melvin; John E. Anson
A mirror presentation and the ensuing species-specific aggressive display served to reinforce the response of swimming through an aperture in paradise fish. Learning and extinction curves were shown with this type of reinforcement; however, non-contingent mirror presentations did not maintain performance. A declining trend in operant swimming performance with repeated image/display reinforcements reflected some habituation of the display.
Behavior Therapy | 1973
Henry C. Rickard; Kenneth B. Melvin; Joe Creel; Laura Creel
The effects of no token, constant tokens, and bonus tokens upon classroomproductivity in a remedial program for behaviorally disturbed children was investigated. A constant level of token reinforcement was sufficient to maintain, but not increase, performance. A dramatic increase in productivity was associated with bonus tokens. The performance of subjects originally intended as a no-token group deteriorated so markedly that it became necessary to institute a token system.
Psychological Record | 1967
Kenneth B. Melvin; F. Thomas Cloar; Lucinda S. Massingill
An attempt was made to imprint 18 Bobwhite Quail chicks 8–20 hours old to a Sparrow Hawk, using a Hess-type circular runway. Seven quail were strongly imprinted to this avian predator.