Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Khairul Anwar Mastor is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Khairul Anwar Mastor.


Psychology and Aging | 2009

Perceptions of Aging across 26 Cultures and their Culture-Level Associates

Corinna E. Löckenhoff; Filip De Fruyt; Antonio Terracciano; Robert R. McCrae; Marleen De Bolle; Paul T. Costa; Maria E. Aguilar-Vafaie; Chang-kyu Ahn; Hyun-nie Ahn; Lidia Alcalay; Jüri Allik; Tatyana V. Avdeyeva; Claudio Barbaranelli; Verónica Benet-Martínez; Marek Blatný; Denis Bratko; Thomas R. Cain; Jarret T. Crawford; Margarida Pedroso de Lima; Emília Ficková; Mirona Gheorghiu; Jamin Halberstadt; Martina Hrebickova; Lee Jussim; Waldemar Klinkosz; Goran Knezevic; Nora Leibovich de Figueroa; Thomas A. Martin; Iris Marušić; Khairul Anwar Mastor

College students (N=3,435) in 26 cultures reported their perceptions of age-related changes in physical, cognitive, and socioemotional areas of functioning and rated societal views of aging within their culture. There was widespread cross-cultural consensus regarding the expected direction of aging trajectories with (a) perceived declines in societal views of aging, physical attractiveness, the ability to perform everyday tasks, and new learning; (b) perceived increases in wisdom, knowledge, and received respect; and (c) perceived stability in family authority and life satisfaction. Cross-cultural variations in aging perceptions were associated with culture-level indicators of population aging, education levels, values, and national character stereotypes. These associations were stronger for societal views on aging and perceptions of socioemotional changes than for perceptions of physical and cognitive changes. A consideration of culture-level variables also suggested that previously reported differences in aging perceptions between Asian and Western countries may be related to differences in population structure.


Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2013

Need Satisfaction and Well-Being Testing Self-Determination Theory in Eight Cultures

A. Timothy Church; Marcia S. Katigbak; Kenneth D. Locke; Hengsheng Zhang; Jiliang Shen; José de Jesús Vargas-Flores; Joselina Ibáñez-Reyes; Junko Tanaka-Matsumi; G.J. Curtis; Helena F. Cabrera; Khairul Anwar Mastor; Juan M. Alvarez; Fernando A. Ortiz; Jean Yves R Simon; Charles M. Ching

According to Self-Determination Theory (SDT), satisfaction of needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness is a universal requirement for psychological well-being. We tested this hypothesis with college students in the United States, Australia, Mexico, Venezuela, the Philippines, Malaysia, China, and Japan. Participants rated the extent to which these needs, plus needs for self-actualization and pleasure-stimulation, were satisfied in various roles and reported their general hedonic (i.e., positive and negative affect) and eudaimonic (e.g., meaning in life, personal growth) well-being. Asian participants averaged lower than non-Asian participants in perceived satisfaction of autonomy, competence, and self-actualization needs and in most aspects of eudaimonic well-being, and these differences were partially accounted for by differences in dialecticism and independent self-construals. Nonetheless, perceived need satisfaction predicted overall well-being to a similar degree in all cultures and in most cultures provided incremental prediction beyond the Big Five traits. Perceived imbalance in the satisfaction of different needs also modestly predicted well-being, particularly negative affect. The study extended support for the universal importance of SDT need satisfaction to several new cultures.


Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2006

Implicit Theories and Self-Perceptions of Traitedness Across Cultures Toward Integration of Cultural and Trait Psychology Perspectives

A. Timothy Church; Marcia S. Katigbak; Alicia M. del Prado; Fernando A. Ortiz; Khairul Anwar Mastor; Yu Harumi; Junko Tanaka-Matsumi; José de Jesús Vargas-Flores; Joselina Ibáñez-Reyes; Fiona A. White; Lilia G. Miramontes; Jose Alberto S. Reyes; Helena F. Cabrera

From the trait perspective, traitedness, or consistency of behavior, is expected in all cultures. However, cultural psychologists argue that behavior may be more determined by traits in individualistic than collectivistic cultures. The authors investigated implicit theories and self-perceptions of traitedness in two individualistic cultures, the United States (n = 342) and Australia (n = 172), and four collectivistic cultures, Mexico (n = 400), Philippines (n = 363), Malaysia (n = 251), and Japan (n = 192). Although implicit trait beliefs were endorsed in all cultural groups, they were stronger in individualistic than collectivistic cultures. Cultural differences in self-perceptions of one’s own traitedness, as operationalized by self-monitoring, were also found, and comparisons involving the United States and most collectivistic cultures were consistent with cultural psychology perspectives. The ability of self-construals to predict implicit beliefs and self-perceptions of traitedness was also investigated. Overall, the results supported efforts to integrate trait and cultural psychology perspectives.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2008

Culture, cross-role consistency, and adjustment: testing trait and cultural psychology perspectives.

A. Timothy Church; Cheryl A. Anderson-Harumi; Alicia M. del Prado; G.J. Curtis; Junko Tanaka-Matsumi; José L. Valdez Medina; Khairul Anwar Mastor; Fiona A. White; Lilia A. Miramontes; Marcia S. Katigbak

Trait and cultural psychology perspectives on cross-role consistency and its relation to adjustment were examined in 2 individualistic cultures, the United States (N=231) and Australia (N=195), and 4 collectivistic cultures, Mexico (N=199), the Philippines (N=195), Malaysia (N=217), and Japan (N=180). Cross-role consistency in trait ratings was evident in all cultures, supporting trait perspectives. Cultural comparisons of mean consistency provided support for cultural psychology perspectives as applied to East Asian cultures (i.e., Japan) but not collectivistic cultures more generally. Some but not all of the hypothesized predictors of consistency were supported across cultures. Cross-role consistency predicted aspects of adjustment in all cultures, but prediction was most reliable in the U.S. sample and weakest in the Japanese sample. Alternative constructs proposed by cultural psychologists--personality coherence, social appraisal, and relationship harmony--predicted adjustment in all cultures but were not, as hypothesized, better predictors of adjustment in collectivistic cultures than in individualistic cultures.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2012

Stereotypes of Age Differences in Personality Traits: Universal and Accurate?

Wayne Chan; Robert R. McCrae; Filip De Fruyt; Lee Jussim; Corinna E. Löckenhoff; Marleen De Bolle; Paul T. Costa; Angelina R. Sutin; Anu Realo; Jüri Allik; Katsuharu Nakazato; Yoshiko Shimonaka; Martina Hřebíčková; Sylvie Graf; Michelle Yik; Marina Brunner-Sciarra; Nora Leibovich de Figueora; Vanina Schmidt; Chang kyu Ahn; Hyun nie Ahn; Maria E. Aguilar-Vafaie; Jerzy Siuta; Barbara Szmigielska; Thomas R. Cain; Jarret T. Crawford; Khairul Anwar Mastor; Jean Pierre Rolland; Florence Nansubuga; Daniel R. Miramontez; Verónica Benet-Martínez

Age trajectories for personality traits are known to be similar across cultures. To address whether stereotypes of age groups reflect these age-related changes in personality, we asked participants in 26 countries (N = 3,323) to rate typical adolescents, adults, and old persons in their own country. Raters across nations tended to share similar beliefs about different age groups; adolescents were seen as impulsive, rebellious, undisciplined, preferring excitement and novelty, whereas old people were consistently considered lower on impulsivity, activity, antagonism, and Openness. These consensual age group stereotypes correlated strongly with published age differences on the five major dimensions of personality and most of 30 specific traits, using as criteria of accuracy both self-reports and observer ratings, different survey methodologies, and data from up to 50 nations. However, personal stereotypes were considerably less accurate, and consensual stereotypes tended to exaggerate differences across age groups.


Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 2012

The Effect of Response Style on Self-Reported Conscientiousness Across 20 Countries:

René Mõttus; Jueri Allik; Anu Realo; Jérôme Rossier; Gregory Zecca; Jennifer Ah-Kion; Denis Amoussou-Yeye; Martin Bäckström; Rasa Barkauskiene; Oumar Barry; Uma Bhowon; Fredrik Björklund; Aleksandra Bochaver; Konstantin Bochaver; Gideon P. de Bruin; Helena F. Cabrera; Sylvia Xiaohua Chen; A. Timothy Church; Daouda Dougoumalé Cissé; Donatien Dahourou; Xiaohang Feng; Yanjun Guan; Hyisung C. Hwang; Fazilah Idris; Marcia S. Katigbak; Peter Kuppens; Anna Kwiatkowska; Alfredas Laurinavičius; Khairul Anwar Mastor; David Matsumoto

Rankings of countries on mean levels of self-reported Conscientiousness continue to puzzle researchers. Based on the hypothesis that cross-cultural differences in the tendency to prefer extreme response categories of ordinal rating scales over moderate categories can influence the comparability of self-reports, this study investigated possible effects of response style on the mean levels of self-reported Conscientiousness in 22 samples from 20 countries. Extreme and neutral responding were estimated based on respondents’ ratings of 30 hypothetical people described in short vignettes. In the vignette ratings, clear cross-sample differences in extreme and neutral responding emerged. These responding style differences were correlated with mean self-reported Conscientiousness scores. Correcting self-reports for extreme and neutral responding changed sample rankings of Conscientiousness, as well as the predictive validities of these rankings for external criteria. The findings suggest that the puzzling country rankings of self-reported Conscientiousness may to some extent result from differences in response styles.


Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2015

Cross-Cultural Differences in a Global “Survey of World Views”

Gerard Saucier; Judith Kenner; Kathryn Iurino; Philippe Bou Malham; Zhuo Chen; Amber Gayle Thalmayer; Markus Kemmelmeier; William Tov; Rachid Boutti; Henok Metaferia; Banu Çankaya; Khairul Anwar Mastor; Kung Yu Hsu; Rongxian Wu; M. Maniruzzaman; Janvier Rugira; Ioannis Tsaousis; Oleg Sosnyuk; Jyoti Regmi Adhikary; Katarzyna Skrzypińska; Boonmee Poungpet; John Maltby; Maria Guadalupe C. Salanga; Adriana Racca; Atsushi Oshio; Elsie Italia; Anastassiya Kovaleva; Masanobu Nakatsugawa; Fabia Morales-Vives; Víctor M. Ruiz

We know that there are cross-cultural differences in psychological variables, such as individualism/collectivism. But it has not been clear which of these variables show relatively the greatest differences. The Survey of World Views project operated from the premise that such issues are best addressed in a diverse sampling of countries representing a majority of the world’s population, with a very large range of item-content. Data were collected online from 8,883 individuals (almost entirely college students based on local publicizing efforts) in 33 countries that constitute more than two third of the world’s population, using items drawn from measures of nearly 50 variables. This report focuses on the broadest patterns evident in item data. The largest differences were not in those contents most frequently emphasized in cross-cultural psychology (e.g., values, social axioms, cultural tightness), but instead in contents involving religion, regularity-norm behaviors, family roles and living arrangements, and ethnonationalism. Content not often studied cross-culturally (e.g., materialism, Machiavellianism, isms dimensions, moral foundations) demonstrated moderate-magnitude differences. Further studies are needed to refine such conclusions, but indications are that cross-cultural psychology may benefit from casting a wider net in terms of the psychological variables of focus.


Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2014

Gender Stereotypes of Personality: Universal and Accurate?

Corinna E. Löckenhoff; Wayne Chan; Robert R. McCrae; Filip De Fruyt; Lee Jussim; Marleen De Bolle; Paul T. Costa; Angelina R. Sutin; Anu Realo; Jüri Allik; Katsuharu Nakazato; Yoshiko Shimonaka; Martina Hřebíčková; Sylvie Graf; Michelle Yik; Emília Ficková; Marina Brunner-Sciarra; Nora Leibovich de Figueora; Vanina Schmidt; Chang kyu Ahn; Hyun nie Ahn; Maria E. Aguilar-Vafaie; Jerzy Siuta; Barbara Szmigielska; Thomas R. Cain; Jarret T. Crawford; Khairul Anwar Mastor; Jean Pierre Rolland; Florence Nansubuga; Daniel R. Miramontez

Numerous studies have documented subtle but consistent sex differences in self-reports and observer-ratings of five-factor personality traits, and such effects were found to show well-defined developmental trajectories and remarkable similarity across nations. In contrast, very little is known about perceived gender differences in five-factor traits in spite of their potential implications for gender biases at the interpersonal and societal level. In particular, it is not clear how perceived gender differences in five-factor personality vary across age groups and national contexts and to what extent they accurately reflect assessed sex differences in personality. To address these questions, we analyzed responses from 3,323 individuals across 26 nations (mean age = 22.3 years, 31% male) who were asked to rate the five-factor personality traits of typical men or women in three age groups (adolescent, adult, and older adult) in their respective nations. Raters perceived women as slightly higher in openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness as well as some aspects of extraversion and neuroticism. Perceived gender differences were fairly consistent across nations and target age groups and mapped closely onto assessed sex differences in self- and observer-rated personality. Associations between the average size of perceived gender differences and national variations in sociodemographic characteristics, value systems, or gender equality did not reach statistical significance. Findings contribute to our understanding of the underlying mechanisms of gender stereotypes of personality and suggest that perceptions of actual sex differences may play a more important role than culturally based gender roles and socialization processes.


European Journal of Personality | 2012

Comparability of self-reported conscientiousness across 21 countries

René Mõttus; Jueri Allik; Anu Realo; Helle Pullmann; Jérôme Rossier; Gregory Zecca; Jennifer Ah-Kion; Denis Amoussou-Yeye; Martin Bäckström; Rasa Barkauskiene; Oumar Barry; Uma Bhowon; Fredrik Björklund; Aleksandra Bochaver; Konstantin Bochaver; Gideon P. de Bruin; Helena F. Cabrera; Sylvia Xiaohua Chen; A. Timothy Church; Daouda Dougoumalé Cissé; Donatien Dahourou; Xiaohang Feng; Yanjun Guan; Hyisung C. Hwang; Fazilah Idris; Marcia S. Katigbak; Peter Kuppens; Anna Kwiatkowska; Alfredas Laurinavičius; Khairul Anwar Mastor

In cross–national studies, mean levels of self–reported phenomena are often not congruent with more objective criteria. One prominent explanation for such findings is that people make self–report judgements in relation to culture–specific standards (often called the reference group effect), thereby undermining the cross–cultural comparability of the judgements. We employed a simple method called anchoring vignettes in order to test whether people from 21 different countries have varying standards for Conscientiousness, a Big Five personality trait that has repeatedly shown unexpected nation–level relationships with external criteria. Participants rated their own Conscientiousness and that of 30 hypothetical persons portrayed in short vignettes. The latter type of ratings was expected to reveal individual differences in standards of Conscientiousness. The vignettes were rated relatively similarly in all countries, suggesting no substantial culture–related differences in standards for Conscientiousness. Controlling for the small differences in standards did not substantially change the rankings of countries on mean self–ratings or the predictive validities of these rankings for objective criteria. These findings are not consistent with mean self–rated Conscientiousness scores being influenced by culture–specific standards. The technique of anchoring vignettes can be used in various types of studies to assess the potentially confounding effects of reference levels. Copyright


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2015

The emergence of sex differences in personality traits in early adolescence: A cross-sectional, cross-cultural study

Marleen De Bolle; Filip De Fruyt; Robert R. McCrae; Corinna E. Löckenhoff; Paul T. Costa; Maria E. Aguilar-Vafaie; Chang-kyu Ahn; Hyun-nie Ahn; Lidia Alcalay; Jüri Allik; Tatyana V. Avdeyeva; Denis Bratko; Marina Brunner-Sciarra; Thomas R. Cain; Wayne Chan; Niyada Chittcharat; Jarret T. Crawford; Ryan Fehr; Emília Ficková; Michele J. Gelfand; Sylvie Graf; Sami Gülgöz; Martina Hřebíčková; Lee Jussim; Waldemar Klinkosz; Goran Knežević; Nora Leibovich de Figueroa; Margarida Pedroso de Lima; Thomas A. Martin; Iris Marušić

Although large international studies have found consistent patterns of sex differences in personality traits among adults (i.e., women scoring higher on most facets), less is known about cross-cultural sex differences in adolescent personality and the role of culture and age in shaping them. The present study examines the NEO Personality Inventory-3 (McCrae, Costa, & Martin, 2005) informant ratings of adolescents from 23 cultures (N = 4,850), and investigates culture and age as sources of variability in sex differences of adolescents personality. The effect for Neuroticism (with females scoring higher than males) begins to take on its adult form around age 14. Girls score higher on Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness at all ages between 12 and 17 years. A more complex pattern emerges for Extraversion and Agreeableness, although by age 17, sex differences for these traits are highly similar to those observed in adulthood. Cross-sectional data suggest that (a) with advancing age, sex differences found in adolescents increasingly converge toward adult patterns with respect to both direction and magnitude; (b) girls display sex-typed personality traits at an earlier age than boys; and (c) the emergence of sex differences was similar across cultures. Practical implications of the present findings are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).

Collaboration


Dive into the Khairul Anwar Mastor's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Fazilah Idris

National University of Malaysia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rosseni Din

National University of Malaysia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

A. Timothy Church

Washington State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marcia S. Katigbak

Washington State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robert R. McCrae

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge