Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Kimberly J. McCarthy is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Kimberly J. McCarthy.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2012

Defining the developmental parameters of temper loss in early childhood: implications for developmental psychopathology

Lauren S. Wakschlag; Seung W. Choi; Alice S. Carter; Heide Hullsiek; James L. Burns; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Ellen Leibenluft

BACKGROUND Temper modulation problems are both a hallmark of early childhood and a common mental health concern. Thus, characterizing specific behavioral manifestations of temper loss along a dimension from normative misbehaviors to clinically significant problems is an important step toward identifying clinical thresholds. METHODS Parent-reported patterns of temper loss were delineated in a diverse community sample of preschoolers (n = 1,490). A developmentally sensitive questionnaire, the Multidimensional Assessment of Preschool Disruptive Behavior (MAP-DB), was used to assess temper loss in terms of tantrum features and anger regulation. Specific aims were: (a) document the normative distribution of temper loss in preschoolers from normative misbehaviors to clinically concerning temper loss behaviors, and test for sociodemographic differences; (b) use Item Response Theory (IRT) to model a Temper Loss dimension; and (c) examine associations of temper loss and concurrent emotional and behavioral problems. RESULTS Across sociodemographic subgroups, a unidimensional Temper Loss model fit the data well. Nearly all (83.7%) preschoolers had tantrums sometimes but only 8.6% had daily tantrums. Normative misbehaviors occurred more frequently than clinically concerning temper loss behaviors. Milder behaviors tended to reflect frustration in expectable contexts, whereas clinically concerning problem indicators were unpredictable, prolonged, and/or destructive. In multivariate models, Temper Loss was associated with emotional and behavioral problems. CONCLUSIONS Parent reports on a developmentally informed questionnaire, administered to a large and diverse sample, distinguished normative and problematic manifestations of preschool temper loss. A developmental, dimensional approach shows promise for elucidating the boundaries between normative early childhood temper loss and emergent psychopathology.


Journal of Traumatic Stress | 2010

Prevalence of exposure to potentially traumatic events in a healthy birth cohort of very young children in the northeastern United States

Julian D. Ford; Lisa A. Fraleigh; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Alice S. Carter

Prevalence estimates of very young childrens exposure to potentially traumatic events (PTEs) are limited. The study objective was to estimate the lifetime prevalence and correlates of noninterpersonal PTEs and violence exposure in a representative healthy birth cohort (ages 1-3 years) from an urban-suburban region of the United States (37.8% minority, 20.2% poverty). Parents completed 2 surveys approximately 1-year apart. By 24-48 months of age, the prevalence of exposure was 26.3% (14.5% noninterpersonal, 13.8% violence). Exposure was common among children living in poverty (49.0% overall, 19.7% noninterpersonal, 33.7% violence). The most consistent factors associated with exposure were poverty, parental depressive symptoms, and single parenting. Findings underscore the potential for prevention and intervention in early childhood to advance public health and reduce morbidity.


Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology | 2016

Can the fear recognition deficits associated with callous-unemotional traits be identified in early childhood?

Stuart F. White; Joel L. Voss; Amélie Petitclerc; Kimberly J. McCarthy; R. James R. Blair; Lauren S. Wakschlag

ABSTRACT Introduction: Callous-unemotional (CU) traits in the presence of conduct problems are associated with increased risk of severe antisocial behavior. Developmentally sensitive methods of assessing CU traits have recently been generated, but their construct validity in relation to neurocognitive underpinnings of CU has not been demonstrated. The current study sought to investigate whether the fear-specific emotion recognition deficits associated with CU traits in older individuals are developmentally expressed in young children as low concern for others and punishment insensitivity. Method: A subsample of 337 preschoolers (mean age 4.8 years, SD = 0.8) who completed neurocognitive tasks was taken from a larger project of preschool psychopathology. Children completed an emotional recognition task in which they were asked to identify the emotional face from the neutral faces in an array. CU traits were assessed using the Low Concern (LC) and Punishment Insensitivity (PI) subscales of the Multidimensional Assessment Profile of Disruptive Behavior (MAP-DB), which were specifically designed to differentiate the normative misbehavior of early childhood from atypical patterns. Results: High LC, but not PI, scores were associated with a fear-specific deficit in emotion recognition. Girls were more accurate than boys in identifying emotional expressions but no significant interaction between LC or PI and sex was observed. Conclusions: Fear recognition deficits associated with CU traits in older individuals were observed in preschoolers with developmentally defined patterns of low concern for others. Confirming that the link between CU-related impairments in empathy and distinct neurocognitive deficits is present in very young children suggests that developmentally specified measurement can detect the substrates of these severe behavioral patterns beginning much earlier than prior work. Exploring the development of CU traits and disruptive behavior disorders at very early ages may provide insights critical to early intervention and prevention of severe antisocial behavior.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2015

Contextual variation in young children's observed disruptive behavior on the DB‐DOS: implications for early identification

Amélie Petitclerc; Ryne Estabrook; James L. Burns; Erica L. Anderson; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Lauren S. Wakschlag

BACKGROUND Contextual variation in child disruptive behavior is well documented but remains poorly understood. We first examine how variation in observed disruptive behavior across interactional contexts is associated with maternal reports of contextual variation in oppositional-defiant behavior and functional impairment. Second, we test whether child inhibitory control explains the magnitude of contextual variation in observed disruptive behavior. METHODS Participants are 497 young children (mean age = 4 years, 11 months) from a subsample of the MAPS, a sociodemographically diverse pediatric sample, enriched for risk of disruptive behavior. Observed anger modulation and behavioral regulation problems were coded on the Disruptive Behavior Diagnostic Observation Schedule (DB-DOS) during interactions with parent and examiner. Oppositional-defiant behavior, and impairment in relationships, with parents and nonparental adults, were measured with the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) interview with the mother. Functional impairment in the home and out-and-about was assessed with the Family Life Impairment Scale (FLIS), and expulsion from child care/school was measured with the baseline survey and FLIS. RESULTS Observed disruptive behavior on the DB-DOS Parent Context was associated with oppositional-defiant behavior with parents, and with impairment at home and out-and-about. Observed disruptive behavior with the Examiner was associated with oppositional-defiant behavior with both parents and nonparental adults, impairment in relationships with nonparental adults, and child care/school expulsion. Differences in observed disruptive behavior in the Parent versus Examiner Contexts was related to the differences in maternal reports of oppositional-defiant behavior with parents versus nonparental adults. Children with larger decreases in disruptive behavior from Parent to Examiner Context had better inhibitory control and fewer attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. CONCLUSIONS The DB-DOS showed clinical utility in a community sample for identifying contextual variation that maps onto reported oppositional-defiant behavior and functioning across contexts. Elucidating the implications of contextual variation for early identification and targeted prevention is an important area for future research.


Child Abuse & Neglect | 2018

Parsing dimensions of family violence exposure in early childhood: Shared and specific contributions to emergent psychopathology and impairment

Ryne Estabrook; David B. Henry; Damion G. Grasso; James L. Burns; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Seth J. Pollak; Lauren S. Wakschlag

Early childhood exposure to family violence predicts increased risk for psychopathology. However, violence between partners and towards children often co-occur. This complicates efforts to determine how experiences of family violence contribute to early mental health problems. Utilizing mother-report data on harsh parenting and intimate partner violence (IPV) from two large community-based, socioeconomically and ethnically diverse samples of 3-5-year-old children, we illustrate the value of a bifactor method for characterizing a family climate in which verbal and physical violence are more chronic and pervasive among family members. In our Calibration sample (N = 1,179), we demonstrate the fit of a bifactor model with a shared violence factor reflecting violence among partners and towards children and orthogonal factors for physically harsh parenting and IPV. Examination of item distributions along quartiles on the identified factors reveals that violent behaviors are most frequent/chronic in families with high scores on the shared violence factor. Next, we apply this model in Validation (N = 1,316) and lab-visit samples (N = 369). Childrens symptoms and impairment showed relatively strong and consistent associations with the shared factor. Some unique associations with IPV and harsh parenting were also observed. Overall, patterns suggest particularly negative impact when verbal and physical violence are more chronic and pervasive among family members. Finally, evidence supporting the bifactor models validity relative to multi-method data from coded interviews about child abuse and IPV and observed parenting is presented. Findings illustrate the value of a bifactor approach for the meaningful characterization of shared and specific features of family violence.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 2010

Prevalence of DSM-IV disorder in a representative, healthy birth cohort at school entry: sociodemographic risks and social adaptation.

Alice S. Carter; Robert J. Wagmiller; Sarah A. O. Gray; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Sarah McCue Horwitz


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2010

Exposure to potentially traumatic events in early childhood: differential links to emergent psychopathology

Alice S. Carter; Roseanne Clark; Marilyn Augustyn; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Julian D. Ford


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2007

A developmental framework for distinguishing disruptive behavior from normative misbehavior in preschool children

Lauren S. Wakschlag; Alice S. Carter; Carri Hill; Barbara Danis; Kate Keenan; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Bennett L. Leventhal


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2015

Attention bias and anxiety in young children exposed to family violence

Seth D. Pollak; Damion J. Grasso; Joel L. Voss; Nicholas D. Mian; Elvira Zobel; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Lauren S. Wakschlag; Daniel S. Pine


Journal of Pediatric Psychology | 2013

Clinical Validity of a Brief Measure of Early Childhood Social–Emotional/Behavioral Problems

Alice S. Carter; Kimberly J. McCarthy; Marilyn Augustyn; Elizabeth Caronna; Roseanne Clark

Collaboration


Dive into the Kimberly J. McCarthy's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alice S. Carter

University of Massachusetts Boston

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Joel L. Voss

Northwestern University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel S. Pine

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Carolyn A. Greene

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David B. Henry

University of Illinois at Chicago

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Elvira Zobel

Northwestern University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge