Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Lauren K. White is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Lauren K. White.


Infant Behavior & Development | 2008

Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy

Brad E. Sheese; Mary K. Rothbart; Michael I. Posner; Lauren K. White; Scott H. Fraundorf

This study investigates early executive attention in infancy by studying the relations between infant sequential looking and other behaviors predictive of later self-regulation. One early marker of executive attention development is anticipatory looking, the act of looking to the location of a target prior to its appearance in that location, a process that involves endogenous control of visual orienting. Previous studies have shown that anticipatory looking is positively related to executive attention as assessed by the ability to resolve spatial conflict in 3-4-year-old children. In the current study, anticipatory looking was positively related to cautious behavioral approach in response to non-threatening novel objects in 6- and 7-month-old infants. This finding and previous findings showing the presence of error detection in infancy are consistent with the hypothesis that there is some degree of executive attention in the first year of life. Anticipatory looking was also related to the frequency of distress, to looking away from disturbing stimuli, and to some self-regulatory behaviors. These results may indicate either early attentional regulation of emotion or close relations between early developing fear and later self-regulation. Overall, the results suggest the presence of rudimentary systems of executive attention in infants and support further studies using anticipatory looking as a measure of individual differences in attention in infancy.


Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2008

Affective primes suppress attention bias to threat in socially anxious individuals

Sarah M. Helfinstein; Lauren K. White; Yair Bar-Haim; Nathan A. Fox

Anxious individuals show an attention bias towards threatening information. However, under conditions of sustained environmental threat this otherwise-present attention bias disappears. It remains unclear whether this suppression of attention bias can be caused by a transient activation of the fear system. In the present experiment, high socially anxious and low socially anxious individuals (HSA group, n=12; LSA group, n=12) performed a modified dot-probe task in which they were shown either a neutral or socially threatening prime word prior to each trial. EEG was collected and ERP components to the prime and faces displays were computed. HSA individuals showed an attention bias to threat after a neutral prime, but no attention bias after a threatening prime, demonstrating that suppression of attention bias can occur after a transient activation of the fear system. LSA individuals showed an opposite pattern: no evidence of a bias to threat with neutral primes but induction of an attention bias to threat following threatening primes. ERP results suggested differential processing of the prime and faces displays by HSA and LSA individuals. However, no group by prime interaction was found for any of ERP components.


Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2011

Cascading effects: The influence of attention bias to threat on the interpretation of ambiguous information

Lauren K. White; Jenna G. Suway; Daniel S. Pine; Yair Bar-Haim; Nathan A. Fox

Both attention bias to threat and negative interpretive bias have been implicated in the emergence and maintenance of anxiety disorders. However, relations between attention and interpretive biases remain poorly understood. The current study experimentally manipulated attention bias to threat and examined the effects of attention training on the way ambiguous information was interpreted. Results suggest that the preferential allocation of attention towards threat affects the manner in which ambiguous information is interpreted. Individuals trained to attend to threat were more likely than individuals in a placebo training group to interpret ambiguous information in a threat-related manner. These data suggest that perturbations in the initial stages of information processing associated with anxiety may lead to a cascade of subsequent processing biases.


Developmental Neuroscience | 2009

Role of Attention in the Regulation of Fear and Anxiety

Lauren K. White; Sarah M. Helfinstein; Bethany C. Reeb-Sutherland; Kathryn A. Degnan; Nathan A. Fox

Emotion regulation makes use of specific aspects of attention and executive functions that are critical for the development of adaptive social functioning, and perturbations in these processes can result in maladaptive behavior and psychopathology. Both involuntary and voluntary attention processes have been examined at both the behavioral and the neural levels and are implicated in the maintenance of fearful or anxious behaviors. However, relatively little is known about how these attention processes come to influence emotional behavior across development. The current review summarizes the extant literature on the links between voluntary and involuntary attention processes and the role that these attention processes have in the etiology, maintenance, and regulation of anxious behavior.


Development and Psychopathology | 2012

Temperamental exuberance and executive function predict propensity for risk taking in childhood.

Ayelet Lahat; Kathryn A. Degnan; Lauren K. White; Jennifer Martin McDermott; Heather A. Henderson; C.W. Lejuez; Nathan A. Fox

The present study utilized a multilevel approach to examine developmental trajectories in risk-taking propensity. We examined the moderating role of specific executive function components, attention shifting and inhibitory control, on the link between exuberant temperament in infancy and propensity for risk taking in childhood. Risk taking was assessed using a task previously associated with sensation seeking and antisocial behaviors. Two hundred ninety-one infants were brought into the lab and behaviors reflecting exuberance were observed at 4, 9, 24, and 36 months of age. Executive function was assessed at 48 months of age. Risk-taking propensity was measured when children were 60 months of age. The results indicated that exuberance and attention shifting, but not inhibitory control, significantly interacted to predict propensity for risk taking. Exuberance was positively associated with risk-taking propensity among children who were relatively low in attention shifting but unrelated for children high in attention shifting. These findings illustrated the multifinality of developmental outcomes for temperamentally exuberant young children and pointed to the distinct regulatory influences of different executive functions for children of differing temperaments. Attention shifting likely affords a child the ability to consider both positive and negative consequences and moderates the relation between early exuberance and risk-taking propensity.


NeuroImage | 2016

Behavioral and neural stability of attention bias to threat in healthy adolescents.

Lauren K. White; Jennifer C. Britton; Stefanie Sequeira; Emily G. Ronkin; Gang Chen; Yair Bar-Haim; Tomer Shechner; Monique Ernst; Nathan A. Fox; Ellen Leibenluft; Daniel S. Pine

Considerable translational research on anxiety examines attention bias to threat and the efficacy of attention training in reducing symptoms. Imaging research on the stability of brain functions engaged by attention bias tasks could inform such research. Perturbed fronto-amygdala function consistently arises in attention bias research on adolescent anxiety. The current report examines the stability of the activation and functional connectivity of these regions on the dot-probe task. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) activation and connectivity data were acquired with the dot-probe task in 39 healthy youth (f=18, Mean Age=13.71years, SD=2.31) at two time points, separated by approximately nine weeks. Intraclass-correlations demonstrate good reliability in both neural activation for the ventrolateral PFC and task-specific connectivity for fronto-amygdala circuitry. Behavioral measures showed generally poor test-retest reliability. These findings suggest potential avenues for future brain imaging work by highlighting brain circuitry manifesting stable functioning on the dot-probe attention bias task.


Brain and Cognition | 2012

Neural activation underlying cognitive control in the context of neutral and affectively charged pictures in children.

Connie Lamm; Lauren K. White; Jennifer Martin McDermott; Nathan A. Fox

The neural correlates of cognitive control for typically developing 9-year-old children were examined using dense-array ERPs and estimates of cortical activation (LORETA) during a go/no-go task with two conditions: a neutral picture condition and an affectively charged picture condition. Activation was estimated for the entire cortex after which data were exported for four regions of interests (ROIs): ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal cortex (OFC/VMPFC). Results revealed faster reaction times, greater N2 activation, and greater prefrontal activation for the affectively charged picture condition than the neutral picture condition. The findings are discussed in reference to the impact of affective stimuli on recruitment of specific brain regions involved in cognitive control.


NeuroImage | 2017

Development of the error-monitoring system from ages 9–35: Unique insight provided by MRI-constrained source localization of EEG

George A. Buzzell; John E. Richards; Lauren K. White; Tyson V. Barker; Daniel S. Pine; Nathan A. Fox

&NA; The ability to self‐detect errors and dynamically adapt behavior is a cornerstone of higher‐level cognition, requiring coordinated activity from a network of neural regions. However, disagreement exists over how the error‐monitoring system develops throughout adolescence and early adulthood. The present report leveraged MRI‐constrained EEG source localization to detail typical development of the error‐monitoring system in a sample of 9–35 year‐olds (n = 43). Participants performed a flanker task while high‐density EEG was recorded; structural MRIs were also acquired for all participants. Analysis of the scalp‐recorded EEG data revealed a frontocentral negativity (error‐related negativity; ERN) immediately following errors for all participants, although the topography of the ERN varied with age. Source localization of the ERN time range revealed maximal activity within the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) for all ages, consistent with recent evidence that the PCC provides a substantial contribution to the scalp‐recorded ERN. Activity within a network of brain regions, including dorsal anterior cingulate, PCC, and parietal cortex, was predictive of improved performance following errors, regardless of age. However, additional activity within insula, orbitofrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus linearly increased with age. Together, these data suggest that the core error‐monitoring system is online by early adolescence and remains relatively stable into adulthood. However, additional brain regions become embedded within this core network with age. These results serve as a model of typical development of the error‐monitoring system from early adolescence into adulthood. HighlightsERN topography and source activity changed as a function of age (9–35 years).Two primary clusters of neural activity found to give rise to the ERN.Dorsal cluster stable across age, ventral‐frontal cluster increased with age.Dorsal cluster (including posterior cingulate) predicted control after errors.Data serve as model of typical error‐monitoring development.


American Journal of Psychiatry | 2017

Complementary Features of Attention Bias Modification Therapy and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy in Pediatric Anxiety Disorders

Lauren K. White; Stefanie Sequeira; Jennifer C. Britton; Melissa A. Brotman; Andrea L. Gold; Erin Berman; Kenneth Towbin; Rany Abend; Nathan A. Fox; Yair Bar-Haim; Ellen Leibenluft; Daniel S. Pine

OBJECTIVE In the treatment of anxiety disorders, attention bias modification therapy (ABMT) and cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) may have complementary effects by targeting different aspects of perturbed threat responses and behaviors. ABMT may target rapid, implicit threat reactions, whereas CBT may target slowly deployed threat responses. The authors used amygdala-based connectivity during a threat-attention task and a randomized controlled trial design to evaluate potential complementary features of these treatments in pediatric anxiety disorders. METHOD Prior to treatment, youths (8-17 years old) with anxiety disorders (N=54), as well as healthy comparison youths (N=51), performed a threat-attention task during functional MRI acquisition. Task-related amygdala-based functional connectivity was assessed. Patients with and without imaging data (N=85) were then randomly assigned to receive CBT paired with either active or placebo ABMT. Clinical response was evaluated, and pretreatment amygdala-based connectivity profiles were compared among patients with varying levels of clinical response. RESULTS Compared with the CBT plus placebo ABMT group, the CBT plus active ABMT group exhibited less severe anxiety after treatment. The patient and healthy comparison groups differed in amygdala-insula connectivity during the threat-attention task. Patients whose connectivity profiles were most different from those of the healthy comparison group exhibited the poorest response to treatment, particularly those who received CBT plus placebo ABMT. CONCLUSIONS The study provides evidence of enhanced clinical effects for patients receiving active ABMT. Moreover, ABMT appears to be most effective for patients with abnormal amygdala-insula connectivity. ABMT may target specific threat processes associated with dysfunctional amygdala-insula connectivity that are not targeted by CBT alone. This may explain the observation of enhanced clinical response to CBT plus active ABMT.


Journal of Experimental Psychopathology | 2016

The cognitive and emotional effects of cognitive bias modification in interpretations in behaviorally inhibited youth

Lauren K. White; Jenna G. Suway; Daniel S. Pine; Andy P. Field; Kathryn J. Lester; Peter Muris; Yair Bar-Haim; Nathan A. Fox

Cognitive bias modification (CBM) procedures follow from the view that interpretive biases play an important role in the development and maintenance of anxiety. As such, understanding the link between interpretive biases and anxiety in youth at risk for anxiety (e.g., behaviorally inhibited children) could elucidate the mechanisms involved in the development of pediatric anxiety. However, to date, the majority of CBM-I work only studies adult populations. The present article presents the results of a CBM study examining effects of positive interpretive bias modification on mood, stress vulnerability, and threat-related attention bias in a group of behaviorally inhibited children (n = 45). Despite successful modification of interpretive bias in the at-risk youth, minimal effects on stress vulnerability or threat-related attention bias were found. The current findings highlight the need for continued research on cognitive biases in anxiety.

Collaboration


Dive into the Lauren K. White's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel S. Pine

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ellen Leibenluft

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jennifer Martin McDermott

University of Massachusetts Amherst

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Melissa A. Brotman

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Stefanie Sequeira

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel H. Wolf

University of Pennsylvania

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge