Leonid Fedorov
University of Tübingen
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Leonid Fedorov.
international conference on neural computation theory and applications | 2016
Leonid Fedorov; Joris Vangeneugden; Martin Giese
Body motion perception from impoverished stimuli shows interesting dynamic properties, such as multistability and spontaneous perceptual switching. Psychophysical experiments show that such multistability disappears when the stimulus includes also shading cues along the body surface. Classical neural models for body motion perception have not addressed perceptual multistability. We present an extension of a classical neurodynamic model for biological and body motion perception that accounts for perceptual switching, and its dependence on shading cues on the body surface. We demonstrate that a set of psychophysical observations can be accounted for in a unifying manner by a hierarchical neural model for body motion processing that includes an additional shading pathway, which processes luminance gradients within the individual body segments. The goal of our model is to explain psychophysics and neural mechanism in the brain.
Scientific Reports | 2018
Leonid Fedorov; Tjeerd M. H. Dijkstra; Martin A. Giese
The visual system is able to recognize body motion from impoverished stimuli. This requires combining stimulus information with visual priors. We present a new visual illusion showing that one of these priors is the assumption that bodies are typically illuminated from above. A change of illumination direction from above to below flips the perceived locomotion direction of a biological motion stimulus. Control experiments show that the underlying mechanism is different from shape-from-shading and directly combines information about body motion with a lighting-from-above prior. We further show that the illusion is critically dependent on the intrinsic luminance gradients of the most mobile parts of the moving body. We present a neural model with physiologically plausible mechanisms that accounts for the illusion and shows how the illumination prior might be encoded within the visual pathway. Our experiments demonstrate, for the first time, a direct influence of illumination priors in high-level motion vision.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2018
Leonid Fedorov; Dong Seon Chang; Martin A. Giese; Hh Bülthoff; Stephan de la Rosa
Significance Why is it so easy for humans to interact with each other? In social interactions, humans coordinate their actions with each other nonverbally. For example, dance partners need to relate their actions to each other to coordinate their movements. The underlying neurocognitive mechanisms supporting this ability are surprisingly poorly understood. We show that human brain processes are sensitive to pairs of matching actions that make up a social interaction. These findings provide insights into the perceptual architecture that helps humans to relate actions to each other. This capability is essential for social interactions, and its understanding will aid future development of therapies to treat social cognitive disorders. A hallmark of human social behavior is the effortless ability to relate one’s own actions to that of the interaction partner, e.g., when stretching out one’s arms to catch a tripping child. What are the behavioral properties of the neural substrates that support this indispensable human skill? Here we examined the processes underlying the ability to relate actions to each other, namely the recognition of spatiotemporal contingencies between actions (e.g., a “giving” that is followed by a “taking”). We used a behavioral adaptation paradigm to examine the response properties of perceptual mechanisms at a behavioral level. In contrast to the common view that action-sensitive units are primarily selective for one action (i.e., primary action, e.g., ‘throwing”), we demonstrate that these processes also exhibit sensitivity to a matching contingent action (e.g., “catching”). Control experiments demonstrate that the sensitivity of action recognition processes to contingent actions cannot be explained by lower-level visual features or amodal semantic adaptation. Moreover, we show that action recognition processes are sensitive only to contingent actions, but not to noncontingent actions, demonstrating their selective sensitivity to contingent actions. Our findings show the selective coding mechanism for action contingencies by action-sensitive processes and demonstrate how the representations of individual actions in social interactions can be linked in a unified representation.
Twenty-Fifth Annual Computational Neuroscience Meeting (CNS*2016) | 2016
Leonid Fedorov; Martin A. Giese
Table of contentsA1 Functional advantages of cell-type heterogeneity in neural circuitsTatyana O. SharpeeA2 Mesoscopic modeling of propagating waves in visual cortexAlain DestexheA3 Dynamics and biomarkers of mental disordersMitsuo KawatoF1 Precise recruitment of spiking output at theta frequencies requires dendritic h-channels in multi-compartment models of oriens-lacunosum/moleculare hippocampal interneuronsVladislav Sekulić, Frances K. SkinnerF2 Kernel methods in reconstruction of current sources from extracellular potentials for single cells and the whole brainsDaniel K. Wójcik, Chaitanya Chintaluri, Dorottya Cserpán, Zoltán SomogyváriF3 The synchronized periods depend on intracellular transcriptional repression mechanisms in circadian clocks.Jae Kyoung Kim, Zachary P. Kilpatrick, Matthew R. Bennett, Kresimir JosićO1 Assessing irregularity and coordination of spiking-bursting rhythms in central pattern generatorsIrene Elices, David Arroyo, Rafael Levi, Francisco B. Rodriguez, Pablo VaronaO2 Regulation of top-down processing by cortically-projecting parvalbumin positive neurons in basal forebrainEunjin Hwang, Bowon Kim, Hio-Been Han, Tae Kim, James T. McKenna, Ritchie E. Brown, Robert W. McCarley, Jee Hyun ChoiO3 Modeling auditory stream segregation, build-up and bistabilityJames Rankin, Pamela Osborn Popp, John RinzelO4 Strong competition between tonotopic neural ensembles explains pitch-related dynamics of auditory cortex evoked fieldsAlejandro Tabas, André Rupp, Emili Balaguer-BallesterO5 A simple model of retinal response to multi-electrode stimulationMatias I. Maturana, David B. Grayden, Shaun L. Cloherty, Tatiana Kameneva, Michael R. Ibbotson, Hamish MeffinO6 Noise correlations in V4 area correlate with behavioral performance in visual discrimination taskVeronika Koren, Timm Lochmann, Valentin Dragoi, Klaus ObermayerO7 Input-location dependent gain modulation in cerebellar nucleus neuronsMaria Psarrou, Maria Schilstra, Neil Davey, Benjamin Torben-Nielsen, Volker SteuberO8 Analytic solution of cable energy function for cortical axons and dendritesHuiwen Ju, Jiao Yu, Michael L. Hines, Liang Chen, Yuguo YuO9 C. elegans interactome: interactive visualization of Caenorhabditis elegans worm neuronal networkJimin Kim, Will Leahy, Eli ShlizermanO10 Is the model any good? Objective criteria for computational neuroscience model selectionJustas Birgiolas, Richard C. Gerkin, Sharon M. CrookO11 Cooperation and competition of gamma oscillation mechanismsAtthaphon Viriyopase, Raoul-Martin Memmesheimer, Stan GielenO12 A discrete structure of the brain wavesYuri Dabaghian, Justin DeVito, Luca PerottiO13 Direction-specific silencing of the Drosophila gaze stabilization systemAnmo J. Kim, Lisa M. Fenk, Cheng Lyu, Gaby MaimonO14 What does the fruit fly think about values? A model of olfactory associative learningChang Zhao, Yves Widmer, Simon Sprecher,Walter SennO15 Effects of ionic diffusion on power spectra of local field potentials (LFP)Geir Halnes, Tuomo Mäki-Marttunen, Daniel Keller, Klas H. Pettersen,Ole A. Andreassen, Gaute T. EinevollO16 Large-scale cortical models towards understanding relationship between brain structure abnormalities and cognitive deficitsYasunori YamadaO17 Spatial coarse-graining the brain: origin of minicolumnsMoira L. Steyn-Ross, D. Alistair Steyn-RossO18 Modeling large-scale cortical networks with laminar structureJorge F. Mejias, John D. Murray, Henry Kennedy, Xiao-Jing WangO19 Information filtering by partial synchronous spikes in a neural populationAlexandra Kruscha, Jan Grewe, Jan Benda, Benjamin LindnerO20 Decoding context-dependent olfactory valence in Drosophila Laurent Badel, Kazumi Ohta, Yoshiko Tsuchimoto, Hokto KazamaP1 Neural network as a scale-free network: the role of a hubB. KahngP2 Hemodynamic responses to emotions and decisions using near-infrared spectroscopy optical imagingNicoladie D. TamP3 Phase space analysis of hemodynamic responses to intentional movement directions using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) optical imaging techniqueNicoladie D.Tam, Luca Pollonini, George ZouridakisP4 Modeling jamming avoidance of weakly electric fishJaehyun Soh, DaeEun KimP5 Synergy and redundancy of retinal ganglion cells in predictionMinsu Yoo, S. E. PalmerP6 A neural field model with a third dimension representing cortical depthViviana Culmone, Ingo BojakP7 Network analysis of a probabilistic connectivity model of the Xenopus tadpole spinal cordAndrea Ferrario, Robert Merrison-Hort, Roman BorisyukP8 The recognition dynamics in the brainChang Sub KimP9 Multivariate spike train analysis using a positive definite kernelTaro TezukaP10 Synchronization of burst periods may govern slow brain dynamics during general anesthesiaPangyu JooP11 The ionic basis of heterogeneity affects stochastic synchronyYoung-Ah Rho, Shawn D. Burton, G. Bard Ermentrout, Jaeseung Jeong, Nathaniel N. UrbanP12 Circular statistics of noise in spike trains with a periodic componentPetr MarsalekP14 Representations of directions in EEG-BCI using Gaussian readoutsHoon-Hee Kim, Seok-hyun Moon, Do-won Lee, Sung-beom Lee, Ji-yong Lee, Jaeseung JeongP15 Action selection and reinforcement learning in basal ganglia during reaching movementsYaroslav I. Molkov, Khaldoun Hamade, Wondimu Teka, William H. Barnett, Taegyo Kim, Sergey Markin, Ilya A. RybakP17 Axon guidance: modeling axonal growth in T-Junction assayCsaba Forro, Harald Dermutz, László Demkó, János VörösP19 Transient cell assembly networks encode persistent spatial memoriesYuri Dabaghian, Andrey BabichevP20 Theory of population coupling and applications to describe high order correlations in large populations of interacting neuronsHaiping HuangP21 Design of biologically-realistic simulations for motor controlSergio Verduzco-FloresP22 Towards understanding the functional impact of the behavioural variability of neuronsFilipa Dos Santos, Peter AndrasP23 Different oscillatory dynamics underlying gamma entrainment deficits in schizophreniaChristoph Metzner, Achim Schweikard, Bartosz ZurowskiP24 Memory recall and spike frequency adaptationJames P. Roach, Leonard M. Sander, Michal R. ZochowskiP25 Stability of neural networks and memory consolidation preferentially occur near criticalityQuinton M. Skilling, Nicolette Ognjanovski, Sara J. Aton, Michal ZochowskiP26 Stochastic Oscillation in Self-Organized Critical States of Small Systems: Sensitive Resting State in Neural SystemsSheng-Jun Wang, Guang Ouyang, Jing Guang, Mingsha Zhang, K. Y. Michael Wong, Changsong ZhouP27 Neurofield: a C++ library for fast simulation of 2D neural field modelsPeter A. Robinson, Paula Sanz-Leon, Peter M. Drysdale, Felix Fung, Romesh G. Abeysuriya, Chris J. Rennie, Xuelong ZhaoP28 Action-based grounding: Beyond encoding/decoding in neural codeYoonsuck Choe, Huei-Fang YangP29 Neural computation in a dynamical system with multiple time scalesYuanyuan Mi, Xiaohan Lin, Si WuP30 Maximum entropy models for 3D layouts of orientation selectivityJoscha Liedtke, Manuel Schottdorf, Fred WolfP31 A behavioral assay for probing computations underlying curiosity in rodentsYoriko Yamamura, Jeffery R. WickensP32 Using statistical sampling to balance error function contributions to optimization of conductance-based modelsTimothy Rumbell, Julia Ramsey, Amy Reyes, Danel Draguljić, Patrick R. Hof, Jennifer Luebke, Christina M. WeaverP33 Exploration and implementation of a self-growing and self-organizing neuron network building algorithmHu He, Xu Yang, Hailin Ma, Zhiheng Xu, Yuzhe WangP34 Disrupted resting state brain network in obese subjects: a data-driven graph theory analysisKwangyeol Baek, Laurel S. Morris, Prantik Kundu, Valerie VoonP35 Dynamics of cooperative excitatory and inhibitory plasticityEverton J. Agnes, Tim P. VogelsP36 Frequency-dependent oscillatory signal gating in feed-forward networks of integrate-and-fire neuronsWilliam F. Podlaski, Tim P. VogelsP37 Phenomenological neural model for adaptation of neurons in area ITMartin Giese, Pradeep Kuravi, Rufin VogelsP38 ICGenealogy: towards a common topology of neuronal ion channel function and genealogy in model and experimentAlexander Seeholzer, William Podlaski, Rajnish Ranjan, Tim VogelsP39 Temporal input discrimination from the interaction between dynamic synapses and neural subthreshold oscillationsJoaquin J. Torres, Fabiano Baroni, Roberto Latorre, Pablo VaronaP40 Different roles for transient and sustained activity during active visual processingBart Gips, Eric Lowet, Mark J. Roberts, Peter de Weerd, Ole Jensen, Jan van der EerdenP41 Scale-free functional networks of 2D Ising model are highly robust against structural defects: neuroscience implicationsAbdorreza Goodarzinick, Mohammad D. Niry, Alireza ValizadehP42 High frequency neuron can facilitate propagation of signal in neural networksAref Pariz, Shervin S. Parsi, Alireza ValizadehP43 Investigating the effect of Alzheimer’s disease related amyloidopathy on gamma oscillations in the CA1 region of the hippocampusJulia M. Warburton, Lucia Marucci, Francesco Tamagnini, Jon Brown, Krasimira Tsaneva-AtanasovaP44 Long-tailed distributions of inhibitory and excitatory weights in a balanced network with eSTDP and iSTDPFlorence I. Kleberg, Jochen TrieschP45 Simulation of EMG recording from hand muscle due to TMS of motor cortexBahar Moezzi, Nicolangelo Iannella, Natalie Schaworonkow, Lukas Plogmacher, Mitchell R. Goldsworthy, Brenton Hordacre, Mark D. McDonnell, Michael C. Ridding, Jochen TrieschP46 Structure and dynamics of axon network formed in primary cell cultureMartin Zapotocky, Daniel Smit, Coralie Fouquet, Alain TrembleauP47 Efficient signal processing and sampling in random networks that generate variabilitySakyasingha Dasgupta, Isao Nishikawa, Kazuyuki Aihara, Taro ToyoizumiP48 Modeling the effect of riluzole on bursting in respiratory neural networksDaniel T. Robb, Nick Mellen, Natalia ToporikovaP49 Mapping relaxation training using effective connectivity analysisRongxiang Tang
Archive | 2016
Leonid Fedorov; Martin A. Giese
Gender differences are well established in cognition and somato-sensation, but there are almost no studies on gender differences in visual perception. One reason is that sample size is often small because effect sizes are large. Small samples are not well suited to test for gender differences. Here, we tested 887 participants from 14 to 90 years old. We tested participants in visual and vernier acuity, visual backward masking and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). We found no gender differences in any of the four tests for younger participants (n = 358; 14–30 years old). Even in a subgroup of schizophrenia patients (n = 260), we did not find gender differences, but large performance deficits in patients compared to controls. For middle-aged participants (n = 170; 31–59 years old), men performed significantly better than women in all perceptual tests, even when we controlled for age. We also found better performance of men compared to women in vernier duration in older participants (n = 99; 60–90 years old) and trends in the same direction for the other tests. Hence, it may be that women’s performance deteriorates with age more strongly than men’s performance. We did not find any difference in WCST, indicating no gender differences for executive functions.Although visual integration is often thought to be retinotopic, visual features can be integrated across retinotopic locations. For example, when a Vernier is followed by a sequence of flanking lines on either side, a percept of two diverging motion streams is elicited. Even though the central Vernier is invisible due to metacontrast masking, its offset is visible in the following elements. If an offset is introduced to one of the flanking lines, the two offsets combine (Otto et al., 2006). Here, by varying the number of flanking lines and the position of the flank offset, we show that this integration lasts up to 450 ms. Furthermore, this process is mandatory, i.e, observers are not able to consciously access the individual lines and change their decision. These results suggest that the contents of consciousness can be modulated by an unconscious memory-process wherein information is integrated for up to 450 ms.The ability of people with Parkinson’s (PwP) to discriminate upright and inverted facial expressions is evaluated using a temporal two-interval forced-choice paradigm. Stimuli are black and white images of neutral, happy, angry, disgusted, fearful, sad and surprised expressions. Inverted stimuli are the two expressions that participants are most and least sensitive to. A range of intensities of expressions (0–100%) are created by morphing between neutral and expressive images. The neutral image (0%) is presented in one interval and the expressive image (varies –100%) in the other. Observers indicate the interval that contained the image that was most expressive. For all upright expressions and all participants, performance increases from chance to 100% correct as intensity of expression increases. Fitted functions describing performance of happy and disgust are shifted to the left of others. This suggests that PwP are most sensitive to expressions of happiness and disgust. PwP and control participants show a small reduction in sensitivity for the expression they are most sensitive to when it is inverted (Face Inversion Effect). For PwP there is a considerable Face Inversion Effect for the expression they are least sensitive to. This suggests that configural face processing is disrupted in Parkinson’s disease.Unlike in cognition, audition and somatosensation, performance between various visual tasks does not correlate. Surprisingly, even tasks that appear similar, like visual acuity and line bisection task do not share much common variance. Similar results were found for visual illusions. For example, the Ebbinghaus and the Muller-Lyer illusions correlate very weakly. The high intra- and inter-observer variability in visual perception is possibly due to perceptual learning, i.e., individual experience shaping perception throughout one’s life time. Here, we studied the relationship between illusion strength and high-level factors such as personality traits (O-Life) and the vividness of mental imagery (VVIQ). In line with previous findings, we found only few correlations between the magnitudes of the visual illusions, despite having high test-retest reliability. More interestingly, we found a high, positive correlation between the magnitude of the Ponzo illusion and vividness of mental imagery. Moreover, the magnitude of the Ponzo illusion was negatively correlated with cognitive disorganization personality trait. These results were specific to the Ponzo-type illusions. Principal component analysis revealed one factor, with high weights mainly on the Ponzo-type illusions, cognitive disorganization and the vividness of mental imagery.Visual backward masking (VBM) is a very sensitive endophenotype of schizophrenia. Masking deficits are highly correlated with reduced EEG amplitudes. In VBM, a target stimulus is followed by a mask, which decreases performance on the target. Here, we investigated the neural correlates of VBM in relatives of schizophrenia patients. We had three conditions: target only and two VBM conditions, with long and short inter-stimulus intervals (ISI). Patients’ performance was impaired, while the relatives performed at the same level as the controls. Interestingly, EEG N1 amplitudes were higher in relatives compared to controls, while they were lower in patients relative to controls as previously reported. For relatives, N1 amplitudes were at the same level in all conditions. For controls and patients, N1 amplitudes increased with task difficult, e.g., amplitudes in the long ISI condition were lower than in short ISI condition. Our results suggest that relatives use a compensation mechanism tuning the brain to maximum performance in all conditions. Since relatives are already at the peak of their activations, increasing the task difficulty does not change brain processing.In crowding, the perception of an object deteriorates in the presence of nearby elements. Obviously, crowding is a ubiquitous phenomenon, since elements are rarely seen in isolation. Despite this ubiquity, there exists no consensus on how to model crowding. In previous experiments, it was shown that the global configuration of the entire stimulus needs to be taken into account. These findings rule out simple pooling models and favor models sensitive to global spatial aspects. In order to further investigate how to incorporate these aspects into models, we tested different types of texture segmentation models such as the Texture Tiling Model, a variation of the LAMINART neural model, a model based on Epitomes, a model based on filtering in the Fourier domain, and several classic neural network models. Across all models, simply capturing regularities in the stimulus does not suffice, as illustrated by a failure of the Fourier analysis model to explain our results. Importantly, we find that models with a grouping mechanism (such as the LAMINART model) work best. However, this grouping may be implemented in different ways, as we will show.Genetic variations of the alpha7 subunit of the nicotinergic acetylcholine receptor gene (CHRNA7) are linked to cognitive deficits in aging and schizophrenia. However, little is known about associations of the CHRNA7 gene with aged-related decline in visual perception. In the present study, we tested whether variations in the alpha7 subunit of the nicotinergic acetylcholine receptor gene (CHRNA7) interact with the perception of coherent motion in healthy aging. We assessed motion coherence for twenty-five older participants (60-73 years) and twenty-six younger participants (20–27 years) for a left/right motion direction discrimination task. A single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) [rs2337980] of the CHRNA7 was genotyped. Overall, 25 participants were classified as T/C allele carriers (11 older), and 22 participants were classified as C/C (11 older). Only 3 participants were T/T and therefore, this group was excluded from further analysis. Overall, older adults had higher motion coherence thresholds than younger adults.We did not find any age-related associations of motion direction discrimination with the CHRNA7. However, regardless of age group, participants carrying the T/C genotype performed the task significantly better than C/C carriers. Our results therefore, indicate a strong relationship between the nicotinic system and motion perception.Reinforcement learning is a type of supervised learning, where reward is sparse and delayed. For example in chess, a series of moves is made until a sparse reward (win, loss) is issued, which makes it impossible to evaluate the value of a single move. Still, there are powerful algorithms, which can learn from delayed and sparse feedback. In order to investigate how visual reinforcement learning is determined by the structure of the RL-problem, we designed a new paradigm, in which we presented an image and asked human observers to choose an action (pushing one out of a number of buttons). The chosen action leads to the next image until observers achieve a goal image. Different learning situations are determined by the image-action matrix, which creates a so-called environment. We first tested whether humans can utilize information learned from a simple environment to solve more complex ones. Results showed no evidence supporting this hypothesis. We then tested our paradigm on several environments with different graph theoretical features, such as regular vs. irregular environments. We found that humans performed better in environments which contain less image-action pairs to the goal. We tested various RL-algorithms and found them to perform inferior to humans.The first psychotic episode is an important period for prevention of cognitive and social deterioration in schizophrenia. Cognitive deficits are of particular interest since they are evident even before a proper diagnosis can be made. Interestingly, there is a relation between cognitive deficits and social functioning. Here, we investigated the changes in cognitive and social functioning during one year and determined also the association of social functioning with cognitive impairments and psychopathological symptoms in first episode patients. 32 patients with a first psychotic episode and 32 healthy controls were investigated. Cognitive functions such as visual perception, executive functions, sustained attention, were tested with visual backward masking (VBM), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), and the Continuous Performance Test (CPT). Follow up tests were carried out after 6 and 12 months. Social functioning of the patients was evaluated by Health and Outcome Scale (HoNOS). Cognitive functions of patients were impaired compared to the healthy controls in all 3 tests. Performance in the cognitive tests did not change significantly during the year. Treatment compliance, however, improved social and symptom indicators.Even in the absence of neurodegenerative disease, aging strongly affects vision. Whereas optical deficits are well documented, much less is known perceptual deficits. In most perceptual studies, one paradigm is tested and it is usually found that older participants perform worse than younger participants. Implicitly, these results are taken as evidence that all visual functions of an individual decline determined by one factor, with some individuals aging more severly than others. However, this is not true. We tested 131 older participants (mean age 70 years old) and 108 younger participants (mean age 22 years old) in 14 perceptual tests (including motion perception, contrast and orientation sensitivity, biological motion perception) and in 3 cognitive tasks (WCST, verbal fluency and digit span). Young participants performed better than older participants in almost all of the tests. However, within the group of older participants, age did not predict performance, i.e., a participant could have good results in biological motion perception but poor results in orientation discrimination. It seems that there is not a single ‘‘aging’’ factor but many.39th European Conference on Visual Perception (ECVP) 2016 Barcelona LEGEND. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Monday August 29th Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Monday August 29th Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 Monday August 29th Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 Tuesday August 30th Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 Tuesday August 30th Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169 Tuesday August 30th Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174 Wednesday August 31th Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190 Wednesday August 31th Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261 Wednesday August 31th Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 264 Thursday September 1st Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 279 Thursday September 1st Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 351 Thursday September 1st Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 353 Author Index. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 370 Perception 2016, Vol. 45(S2) 1–383 ! The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0301006616671273 pec.sagepub.comYoung adults typically display a processing advantage for the left side of space (‘‘pseudoneglect’’), whereas older adults display no strongly lateralised bias, or indeed a preference towards the right (Benwell et al., 2014; Schmitz & Peigneux, 2011). For young adults, we have recently reported that 5 commonly-used spatial attention tasks (line bisection, landmark, greyscales, gratingscales and lateralised visual detection) all provide stable intra-task measures of bias over time, however no strong inter-task correlations were found (Learmonth et al., 2015). At present there is no systematic evidence for intra- and inter-task consistency in older adults. To investigate this, we tested 22 older adults (mean age ¼ 70.44) on these five tasks, on two different days. Preliminary results show that three of the five tasks (line bisection, landmark and grayscales) seem to provide stable measures over testing sessions, indicating that they measure a consistent property of the spatial attention network. However, as per our previous finding in young adults, there seem to be no significant between-task correlations. Moreover, in contrast to the leftward biases reported in young adults, this elderly age group showed no significant lateral biases on any of the tasks.Estimates if the visual speed of human movements such as hand gestures, facial expressions and locomotion are important during social interactions because they can be used to infer mood and intention. However it is not clear how observers use retinal signals to estimate real-world movement speed. We conducted a series of experiments to investigate adaptation-induced changes in apparent human locomotion speed, to test whether the changes show repulsion of similar speeds or global re-normalisation of all apparent speeds. Participants adapted to videos of walking or running figures at various playback speeds, and then judged the apparent movement speed of subsequently presented test clips. Their task was to report whether each test clip appeared to be faster or slower than a ‘natural’ speed. After adaptation to a slow-motion or fast-forward video, psychometric functions showed that the apparent speed of all test clips changed, becoming faster or slower respectively, consistent with global re-normalisation rather than with repulsion of test speeds close to the adapting speed. The adaptation effect depended on the retinal speed of the adapting stimulus but did not require recognizably human movements.Awareness, focused attention, and task-relevance were thought to be necessary for perceptual learning (PL): a Feature of the Stimulus (FoS) on which participants perform a task is learned, while a task-irrelevant FoS is not learned. This view has been challenged by the discovery of taskirrelevant PL, occurring for subthreshold task-irrelevant stimuli presented at an unattended, peripheral location. Here, we proof further evidence for task-irrelevant PL by showing that it can occur for subthreshold task-irrelevant FoS presented in the fovea (hence spatially attended). Our experiment was divided into 3 stages: pre-test, training, and post-test. During pre- and posttests, participants performed a 3-dot Vernier task and a 3-dot bisection task. During training, participants performed an unrelated task (luminance discrimination) on the same stimulus. The task-irrelevant FoS, manipulated during training, was the position of the middle dot: either a subthreshold left/right offset (Experimental Group) or in perfect alignment with the outer dots (Control Group). The Experimental Group showed performance improvements in the Vernier task but not in the bisection task; while the Control Group showed no effect on performance in either task. We suggest that PL can occur as an effect of mere exposure to a subthreshold taskirrelevant FoS, which is spatially attended.Feature fusion reflects temporal integration. Previous studies mostly employed foveal presentations with no attention manipulation. In this study we examined the effects of sustained spatial attention on temporal integration using feature-fusion with peripheral presentation. We used a typical feature fusion display. A vernier and anti-vernier stimuli (vernier with offset in the opposite direction than the first vernier) were presented in rapid succession in one of 2 possible locations, at 2° of eccentricity. The attended condition involved endogenous attention manipulation achieved through holding the location of the stimuli constant for the whole block (i.e., the stimuli were always presented to the right of the fixation). Thus, in this condition there was no spatial uncertainty. In the unattended condition, the stimuli could appear either to the right or left of the fixation with equal probability, generating spatial uncertainty. We found considerable feature fusion in the attended condition, suggesting that feature fusion can also occur with peripheral presentation. However, no feature fusion was found without attention (i.e., when there was uncertainty regarding the stimuli location), suggesting that spatial attention improves temporal integration. We are currently conducting similar experiments using different attentional cues to manipulate transient attention.Crowding refers to the detrimental effect of nearby elements on target perception. Recently, Harrison and Bex (Curr Biol, 2015) modeled performance in a novel orientation crowding paradigm where observers reported the orientation of a Landolt C presented alone or surrounded by a flanking C. They found that crowding decreased as flanker radius increased, and their model fit these results well. A key prediction of their model is that flankers with each radius, if presented simultaneously, will additively deteriorate performance. However, evidence from other paradigms suggests that presenting several flankers can actually improve performance, if configured to group separately from the target (e.g., Manassi et al., J Vis 2012). Here, we show a similar grouping effect in the orientation crowding paradigm. We tested observers in three conditions: no flanker, one flanker, or five aligned flankers. All of our observers experienced less crowding with five aligned flankers than one flanker, and our reproduction of Harrison and Bex’s model indeed produced the opposite result. Although Harrison and Bex’s model provides a powerful framework to explain some crowding phenomena, a truly unifying model must also account for such grouping effects, as they are likely ubiquitous in everyday environments.
Archive | 2016
D-S Chang; Leonid Fedorov; Martin A. Giese; Hh Bülthoff; S de la Rosa
Gender differences are well established in cognition and somato-sensation, but there are almost no studies on gender differences in visual perception. One reason is that sample size is often small because effect sizes are large. Small samples are not well suited to test for gender differences. Here, we tested 887 participants from 14 to 90 years old. We tested participants in visual and vernier acuity, visual backward masking and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). We found no gender differences in any of the four tests for younger participants (n = 358; 14–30 years old). Even in a subgroup of schizophrenia patients (n = 260), we did not find gender differences, but large performance deficits in patients compared to controls. For middle-aged participants (n = 170; 31–59 years old), men performed significantly better than women in all perceptual tests, even when we controlled for age. We also found better performance of men compared to women in vernier duration in older participants (n = 99; 60–90 years old) and trends in the same direction for the other tests. Hence, it may be that women’s performance deteriorates with age more strongly than men’s performance. We did not find any difference in WCST, indicating no gender differences for executive functions.Although visual integration is often thought to be retinotopic, visual features can be integrated across retinotopic locations. For example, when a Vernier is followed by a sequence of flanking lines on either side, a percept of two diverging motion streams is elicited. Even though the central Vernier is invisible due to metacontrast masking, its offset is visible in the following elements. If an offset is introduced to one of the flanking lines, the two offsets combine (Otto et al., 2006). Here, by varying the number of flanking lines and the position of the flank offset, we show that this integration lasts up to 450 ms. Furthermore, this process is mandatory, i.e, observers are not able to consciously access the individual lines and change their decision. These results suggest that the contents of consciousness can be modulated by an unconscious memory-process wherein information is integrated for up to 450 ms.The ability of people with Parkinson’s (PwP) to discriminate upright and inverted facial expressions is evaluated using a temporal two-interval forced-choice paradigm. Stimuli are black and white images of neutral, happy, angry, disgusted, fearful, sad and surprised expressions. Inverted stimuli are the two expressions that participants are most and least sensitive to. A range of intensities of expressions (0–100%) are created by morphing between neutral and expressive images. The neutral image (0%) is presented in one interval and the expressive image (varies –100%) in the other. Observers indicate the interval that contained the image that was most expressive. For all upright expressions and all participants, performance increases from chance to 100% correct as intensity of expression increases. Fitted functions describing performance of happy and disgust are shifted to the left of others. This suggests that PwP are most sensitive to expressions of happiness and disgust. PwP and control participants show a small reduction in sensitivity for the expression they are most sensitive to when it is inverted (Face Inversion Effect). For PwP there is a considerable Face Inversion Effect for the expression they are least sensitive to. This suggests that configural face processing is disrupted in Parkinson’s disease.Unlike in cognition, audition and somatosensation, performance between various visual tasks does not correlate. Surprisingly, even tasks that appear similar, like visual acuity and line bisection task do not share much common variance. Similar results were found for visual illusions. For example, the Ebbinghaus and the Muller-Lyer illusions correlate very weakly. The high intra- and inter-observer variability in visual perception is possibly due to perceptual learning, i.e., individual experience shaping perception throughout one’s life time. Here, we studied the relationship between illusion strength and high-level factors such as personality traits (O-Life) and the vividness of mental imagery (VVIQ). In line with previous findings, we found only few correlations between the magnitudes of the visual illusions, despite having high test-retest reliability. More interestingly, we found a high, positive correlation between the magnitude of the Ponzo illusion and vividness of mental imagery. Moreover, the magnitude of the Ponzo illusion was negatively correlated with cognitive disorganization personality trait. These results were specific to the Ponzo-type illusions. Principal component analysis revealed one factor, with high weights mainly on the Ponzo-type illusions, cognitive disorganization and the vividness of mental imagery.Visual backward masking (VBM) is a very sensitive endophenotype of schizophrenia. Masking deficits are highly correlated with reduced EEG amplitudes. In VBM, a target stimulus is followed by a mask, which decreases performance on the target. Here, we investigated the neural correlates of VBM in relatives of schizophrenia patients. We had three conditions: target only and two VBM conditions, with long and short inter-stimulus intervals (ISI). Patients’ performance was impaired, while the relatives performed at the same level as the controls. Interestingly, EEG N1 amplitudes were higher in relatives compared to controls, while they were lower in patients relative to controls as previously reported. For relatives, N1 amplitudes were at the same level in all conditions. For controls and patients, N1 amplitudes increased with task difficult, e.g., amplitudes in the long ISI condition were lower than in short ISI condition. Our results suggest that relatives use a compensation mechanism tuning the brain to maximum performance in all conditions. Since relatives are already at the peak of their activations, increasing the task difficulty does not change brain processing.In crowding, the perception of an object deteriorates in the presence of nearby elements. Obviously, crowding is a ubiquitous phenomenon, since elements are rarely seen in isolation. Despite this ubiquity, there exists no consensus on how to model crowding. In previous experiments, it was shown that the global configuration of the entire stimulus needs to be taken into account. These findings rule out simple pooling models and favor models sensitive to global spatial aspects. In order to further investigate how to incorporate these aspects into models, we tested different types of texture segmentation models such as the Texture Tiling Model, a variation of the LAMINART neural model, a model based on Epitomes, a model based on filtering in the Fourier domain, and several classic neural network models. Across all models, simply capturing regularities in the stimulus does not suffice, as illustrated by a failure of the Fourier analysis model to explain our results. Importantly, we find that models with a grouping mechanism (such as the LAMINART model) work best. However, this grouping may be implemented in different ways, as we will show.Genetic variations of the alpha7 subunit of the nicotinergic acetylcholine receptor gene (CHRNA7) are linked to cognitive deficits in aging and schizophrenia. However, little is known about associations of the CHRNA7 gene with aged-related decline in visual perception. In the present study, we tested whether variations in the alpha7 subunit of the nicotinergic acetylcholine receptor gene (CHRNA7) interact with the perception of coherent motion in healthy aging. We assessed motion coherence for twenty-five older participants (60-73 years) and twenty-six younger participants (20–27 years) for a left/right motion direction discrimination task. A single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) [rs2337980] of the CHRNA7 was genotyped. Overall, 25 participants were classified as T/C allele carriers (11 older), and 22 participants were classified as C/C (11 older). Only 3 participants were T/T and therefore, this group was excluded from further analysis. Overall, older adults had higher motion coherence thresholds than younger adults.We did not find any age-related associations of motion direction discrimination with the CHRNA7. However, regardless of age group, participants carrying the T/C genotype performed the task significantly better than C/C carriers. Our results therefore, indicate a strong relationship between the nicotinic system and motion perception.Reinforcement learning is a type of supervised learning, where reward is sparse and delayed. For example in chess, a series of moves is made until a sparse reward (win, loss) is issued, which makes it impossible to evaluate the value of a single move. Still, there are powerful algorithms, which can learn from delayed and sparse feedback. In order to investigate how visual reinforcement learning is determined by the structure of the RL-problem, we designed a new paradigm, in which we presented an image and asked human observers to choose an action (pushing one out of a number of buttons). The chosen action leads to the next image until observers achieve a goal image. Different learning situations are determined by the image-action matrix, which creates a so-called environment. We first tested whether humans can utilize information learned from a simple environment to solve more complex ones. Results showed no evidence supporting this hypothesis. We then tested our paradigm on several environments with different graph theoretical features, such as regular vs. irregular environments. We found that humans performed better in environments which contain less image-action pairs to the goal. We tested various RL-algorithms and found them to perform inferior to humans.The first psychotic episode is an important period for prevention of cognitive and social deterioration in schizophrenia. Cognitive deficits are of particular interest since they are evident even before a proper diagnosis can be made. Interestingly, there is a relation between cognitive deficits and social functioning. Here, we investigated the changes in cognitive and social functioning during one year and determined also the association of social functioning with cognitive impairments and psychopathological symptoms in first episode patients. 32 patients with a first psychotic episode and 32 healthy controls were investigated. Cognitive functions such as visual perception, executive functions, sustained attention, were tested with visual backward masking (VBM), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), and the Continuous Performance Test (CPT). Follow up tests were carried out after 6 and 12 months. Social functioning of the patients was evaluated by Health and Outcome Scale (HoNOS). Cognitive functions of patients were impaired compared to the healthy controls in all 3 tests. Performance in the cognitive tests did not change significantly during the year. Treatment compliance, however, improved social and symptom indicators.Even in the absence of neurodegenerative disease, aging strongly affects vision. Whereas optical deficits are well documented, much less is known perceptual deficits. In most perceptual studies, one paradigm is tested and it is usually found that older participants perform worse than younger participants. Implicitly, these results are taken as evidence that all visual functions of an individual decline determined by one factor, with some individuals aging more severly than others. However, this is not true. We tested 131 older participants (mean age 70 years old) and 108 younger participants (mean age 22 years old) in 14 perceptual tests (including motion perception, contrast and orientation sensitivity, biological motion perception) and in 3 cognitive tasks (WCST, verbal fluency and digit span). Young participants performed better than older participants in almost all of the tests. However, within the group of older participants, age did not predict performance, i.e., a participant could have good results in biological motion perception but poor results in orientation discrimination. It seems that there is not a single ‘‘aging’’ factor but many.39th European Conference on Visual Perception (ECVP) 2016 Barcelona LEGEND. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Monday August 29th Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Monday August 29th Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 Monday August 29th Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 Tuesday August 30th Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 Tuesday August 30th Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169 Tuesday August 30th Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174 Wednesday August 31th Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190 Wednesday August 31th Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261 Wednesday August 31th Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 264 Thursday September 1st Poster presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 279 Thursday September 1st Symposia presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 351 Thursday September 1st Oral presentations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 353 Author Index. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 370 Perception 2016, Vol. 45(S2) 1–383 ! The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0301006616671273 pec.sagepub.comYoung adults typically display a processing advantage for the left side of space (‘‘pseudoneglect’’), whereas older adults display no strongly lateralised bias, or indeed a preference towards the right (Benwell et al., 2014; Schmitz & Peigneux, 2011). For young adults, we have recently reported that 5 commonly-used spatial attention tasks (line bisection, landmark, greyscales, gratingscales and lateralised visual detection) all provide stable intra-task measures of bias over time, however no strong inter-task correlations were found (Learmonth et al., 2015). At present there is no systematic evidence for intra- and inter-task consistency in older adults. To investigate this, we tested 22 older adults (mean age ¼ 70.44) on these five tasks, on two different days. Preliminary results show that three of the five tasks (line bisection, landmark and grayscales) seem to provide stable measures over testing sessions, indicating that they measure a consistent property of the spatial attention network. However, as per our previous finding in young adults, there seem to be no significant between-task correlations. Moreover, in contrast to the leftward biases reported in young adults, this elderly age group showed no significant lateral biases on any of the tasks.Estimates if the visual speed of human movements such as hand gestures, facial expressions and locomotion are important during social interactions because they can be used to infer mood and intention. However it is not clear how observers use retinal signals to estimate real-world movement speed. We conducted a series of experiments to investigate adaptation-induced changes in apparent human locomotion speed, to test whether the changes show repulsion of similar speeds or global re-normalisation of all apparent speeds. Participants adapted to videos of walking or running figures at various playback speeds, and then judged the apparent movement speed of subsequently presented test clips. Their task was to report whether each test clip appeared to be faster or slower than a ‘natural’ speed. After adaptation to a slow-motion or fast-forward video, psychometric functions showed that the apparent speed of all test clips changed, becoming faster or slower respectively, consistent with global re-normalisation rather than with repulsion of test speeds close to the adapting speed. The adaptation effect depended on the retinal speed of the adapting stimulus but did not require recognizably human movements.Awareness, focused attention, and task-relevance were thought to be necessary for perceptual learning (PL): a Feature of the Stimulus (FoS) on which participants perform a task is learned, while a task-irrelevant FoS is not learned. This view has been challenged by the discovery of taskirrelevant PL, occurring for subthreshold task-irrelevant stimuli presented at an unattended, peripheral location. Here, we proof further evidence for task-irrelevant PL by showing that it can occur for subthreshold task-irrelevant FoS presented in the fovea (hence spatially attended). Our experiment was divided into 3 stages: pre-test, training, and post-test. During pre- and posttests, participants performed a 3-dot Vernier task and a 3-dot bisection task. During training, participants performed an unrelated task (luminance discrimination) on the same stimulus. The task-irrelevant FoS, manipulated during training, was the position of the middle dot: either a subthreshold left/right offset (Experimental Group) or in perfect alignment with the outer dots (Control Group). The Experimental Group showed performance improvements in the Vernier task but not in the bisection task; while the Control Group showed no effect on performance in either task. We suggest that PL can occur as an effect of mere exposure to a subthreshold taskirrelevant FoS, which is spatially attended.Feature fusion reflects temporal integration. Previous studies mostly employed foveal presentations with no attention manipulation. In this study we examined the effects of sustained spatial attention on temporal integration using feature-fusion with peripheral presentation. We used a typical feature fusion display. A vernier and anti-vernier stimuli (vernier with offset in the opposite direction than the first vernier) were presented in rapid succession in one of 2 possible locations, at 2° of eccentricity. The attended condition involved endogenous attention manipulation achieved through holding the location of the stimuli constant for the whole block (i.e., the stimuli were always presented to the right of the fixation). Thus, in this condition there was no spatial uncertainty. In the unattended condition, the stimuli could appear either to the right or left of the fixation with equal probability, generating spatial uncertainty. We found considerable feature fusion in the attended condition, suggesting that feature fusion can also occur with peripheral presentation. However, no feature fusion was found without attention (i.e., when there was uncertainty regarding the stimuli location), suggesting that spatial attention improves temporal integration. We are currently conducting similar experiments using different attentional cues to manipulate transient attention.Crowding refers to the detrimental effect of nearby elements on target perception. Recently, Harrison and Bex (Curr Biol, 2015) modeled performance in a novel orientation crowding paradigm where observers reported the orientation of a Landolt C presented alone or surrounded by a flanking C. They found that crowding decreased as flanker radius increased, and their model fit these results well. A key prediction of their model is that flankers with each radius, if presented simultaneously, will additively deteriorate performance. However, evidence from other paradigms suggests that presenting several flankers can actually improve performance, if configured to group separately from the target (e.g., Manassi et al., J Vis 2012). Here, we show a similar grouping effect in the orientation crowding paradigm. We tested observers in three conditions: no flanker, one flanker, or five aligned flankers. All of our observers experienced less crowding with five aligned flankers than one flanker, and our reproduction of Harrison and Bex’s model indeed produced the opposite result. Although Harrison and Bex’s model provides a powerful framework to explain some crowding phenomena, a truly unifying model must also account for such grouping effects, as they are likely ubiquitous in everyday environments.
Journal of Vision | 2015
Leonid Fedorov; Martin Giese
UNLABELLED The perception of 3D static shapes from 2D images is strongly influenced by surface shading cues (Brewster, 1847; Ramachandran, 1988; Yamane, 2008). Most research on biological motion perception has focused on the influences of 2D motion and form cues and binocular disparity, while the influence of surface shading has only rarely been addressed. We discovered a new visual illusion, where the light source direction flips the perceived walking direction, indicating a lighting from above prior in biological motion perception. METHODS Our experiments builds on a bistable biological motion stimulus that shows a walker from the front (Vanrie, 2004). Instead of dots we used volumetric conic elements placed at the limb centers. The elevation of the light source direction was varied systematically between 180 degrees (above) to 0 degrees (below). Two stimuli were compared that were derived from the same 3D walker walking either towards or away from the observer. Participants had to report the perceived walking direction. In an additional control experiment we investigated the influence of individual features on the illusion, by removing the gradual shading from all limb segments except for selected subsets. RESULTS The light source position has a strong influence on the perceived walking directions (for walking away: F(16,176)=178.9,p< 0.01; towards: F(16,176)=154.3,p< 0.01), where illumination from below results in a flip of the perceived walking direction by 180 deg. The control experiment shows that effect is mainly driven by the shading gradients of the forearms and the thighs. CONCLUSION Similar to the perception of static shapes, biological motion perception depends on a lighting-from-above prior. We were able to reproduce this effect can by extending a neural model for biological motion perception (Giese, 2003) by an additional shading pathway, which analyzes gradual shading variations within surface elements, while suppressing contrast edges at the borders. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.
BMC Neuroscience | 2015
Martin Giese; Leonid Fedorov; Rufin Vogels
The visual perception of body movements shows interesting dynamical properties. Biological motion perception can show perceptual multi-stability with respect to the perceived walking direction [1]. In addition, body motion perception is subject to adaptation, as is demonstrated by the existence of high-level after-effects and of fMRI repetition suppression in relevant areas. Existing neural models for action recognition do not account for these phenomena. We present neurodynamical model that reproduces these phenomena, and which allows to study the interplay between multi-stability, adaptation and intrinsic fluctuations in body motion perception.
BMC Neuroscience | 2015
Leonid Fedorov; Martin Giese
Point-light or stick-figure biological motion stimuli, due to the absence of depth cues, can induce bistable perception, where the walker is perceived as heading in two alternating directions [1,2]. Psychophysical studies suggested an importance of depth cues for biological motion perception [3]. However, neural models of biological motion perception so far have focused on the processing of features that characterize the 2D structure and motion of the human body [4,5]. We extend such models for the processing of shading cues in order to analyze the three-dimensional structure of walkers from monocular stimuli.
BMC Neuroscience | 2014
Martin A. Giese; Leonid Fedorov
The recognition of body motion requires the temporal integration information over time. This integration likely is achieved by dynamic neural networks, which are composed from neurons that are selective for form and optic flow patterns [1]. Physiological evidence also indicates that many action-selective visual neurons are view-dependent, so that such representations likely represent not only the time structure of actions, but also the stimulus view. The dynamic and self-organization properties of such representations have rarely been studied. We propose a neurodynamical model for the visual encoding of actions that is based on a two-dimensional neural field with the defining equations τuu.(φ,θ,t)=-u(φ,θ,t)+w(φ,θ)*[u(φ,θ,t)]++s(φ,θ,t)-αa(φ,θ,t)+ξ(φ,θ,t)τaȧ(φ,θ,t)=-a(φ,θ,t)+[u(φ,θ,t)]+ u specifies the membrane potential and a the adaptation state. The recurrent interaction kernel w is symmetric with respect to the origin in o-direction and asymmetric in ξ-direction, with an additional strong inhibitory component. The spatial convolution * is periodic. The stimulus signal s models an (idealized) activity distribution of shape- (or optic flow-) selective neurons that are maximally responding to stimulus frame θ and view angle o of an ongoing action stimulus. The noise variable ξ is defined by a Gaussian process whose kernel was fitted in order to reproduce coarsely the correlation statistics, dependent on the tuning similarity of the neurons. Time scales and adaptation strength are specified by the positive constants τu, τvand α. The first equation defines a dynamic neural field that stabilizes a stimulus-locked travelling peak solution in ξ-direction, and a winner-takes-all competition in o-direction. The second equation specifies a simple adaptation process.