Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Lesley Hart is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Lesley Hart.


Journal of Learning Disabilities | 1998

Selective Predictive Value of Rapid Automatized Naming in Poor Readers

Marianne S. Meyer; Frank B. Wood; Lesley Hart; Rebecca H. Felton

This study considers the differential predictive value of rapid naming tests for various aspects of later reading, where the differential is between nondisabled and poor readers. Two large-N longitudinal samples of students who have been evaluated from third through eighth grades are studied: (a) a randomly accessed, normally distributed group including students with varying degrees of reading ability (N = 154), and (b) a group of poor readers whose single-word reading in third grade is at or below the population 10th percentile (N = 64). Outcomes in fifth and eighth grade were measured in both groups. Single-word reading in both grades was strongly predicted from third-grade rapid naming only within the poor readers, even when IQ, socioeconomc status, and third-grade single-word reading were statistically controlled. Although rapid naming had predictive value within the large, normally distributed group, its predictive power was entirely absent in the average-reading nondisabled students who were between the 10th and 90th percentiles (n = 122). The fact that rapid naming has predictive power only for poor readers but not for average readers is interpreted as suggesting that impaired readers are qualitatively different from the normal-reading population and are not simply the “tail” of a normal distribution of reading ability. It also seems that it is the automaticity of retrieval, not the knowledge of names itself (as in confrontational naming tasks), that gives the predictive power in rapid naming. These data are considered in light of the one-and two-factor theories of the underlying processes involved in reading disability or dyslexia.


Brain & Development | 2012

Searching for Potocki–Lupski syndrome phenotype: A patient with language impairment and no autism

A. Gulhan Ercan-Sencicek; Nicole R. Davis Wright; Stephen J. Frost; Robert K. Fulbright; Susan Felsenfeld; Lesley Hart; Nicole Landi; W. Einar Mencl; Stephan J. Sanders; Kenneth R. Pugh; Matthew W. State; Elena L. Grigorenko

Potocki-Lupski syndrome (PTLS; OMIM 610883) is a genomic syndrome that arises as a result of a duplication of 17p11.2. Although numerous cases of individuals with PTLS have been presented in the literature, its behavioral characterization is still ambiguous. We present a male child with a de novo dup(17)(p11.2p11.2) and he does not possess any autistic features, but is characterized by severe speech and language impairment. In the context of the analyses of this patient and other cases of PTLS, we argue that the central feature of the syndrome appears to be related to diminished speech and language capacity, rather than the specific social deficits central to autism.


Journal of Communication Disorders | 2015

Language development of internationally adopted children: Adverse early experiences outweigh the age of acquisition effect.

Natalia Rakhlin; Sascha Hein; Niamh Doyle; Lesley Hart; Donna Macomber; Vladislav Ruchkin; Mei Tan; Elena L. Grigorenko

UNLABELLED We compared English language and cognitive skills between internationally adopted children (IA; mean age at adoption=2.24, SD=1.8) and their non-adopted peers from the US reared in biological families (BF) at two time points. We also examined the relationships between outcome measures and age at initial institutionalization, length of institutionalization, and age at adoption. On measures of general language, early literacy, and non-verbal IQ, the IA group performed significantly below their age-peers reared in biological families at both time points, but the group differences disappeared on receptive vocabulary and kindergarten concept knowledge at the second time point. Furthermore, the majority of children reached normative age expectations between 1 and 2 years post-adoption on all standardized measures. Although the age at adoption, age of institutionalization, length of institutionalization, and time in the adoptive family all demonstrated significant correlations with one or more outcome measures, the negative relationship between length of institutionalization and child outcomes remained most robust after controlling for the other variables. Results point to much flexibility and resilience in childrens capacity for language acquisition as well as the potential primacy of length of institutionalization in explaining individual variation in IA childrens outcomes. LEARNING OUTCOMES (1) Readers will be able to understand the importance of pre-adoption environment on language and early literacy development in internationally adopted children. (2) Readers will be able to compare the strength of the association between the length of institutionalization and language outcomes with the strength of the association between the latter and the age at adoption. (3) Readers will be able to understand that internationally adopted children are able to reach age expectations on expressive and receptive language measures despite adverse early experiences and a replacement of their first language with an adoptive language.


Learning and Individual Differences | 2013

Associations between household responsibilities and academic competencies in the context of education accessibility in Zambia.

Jodi Reich; Sascha Hein; Suzanna Krivulskaya; Lesley Hart; Nina Gumkowski; Elena L. Grigorenko

The relationship between education and socioeconomic status has been demonstrated in studies of the developed and the developing world, yet there are communities in which schooling is either not available to all children or not a preferred activity for all children. In this study, we investigated the differences between children in-school and out-of-school in rural and peri-urban communities of Zambia. As expected, we found that the children in-school performed higher in domains of adaptive behavior and on assessments of academic achievement (i.e., mathematics, reading). Somewhat unexpectedly, however, when controlling for socioeconomic status, home responsibilities (i.e., chores, work) were a positive predictor for the performance of the children out-of-school, but a negative predictor for the children in-school. The relationship between home responsibilities and academic performance may be bidirectional and differential; for example, our findings allow for the hypothesis that for in-school children chores take time away from the studies, but for out-of-school children they provide some limited mathematics exposure.


Journal of Learning Disabilities | 2015

Academic Achievement Among Juvenile Detainees

Elena L. Grigorenko; Donna Macomber; Lesley Hart; Adam Naples; John Chapman; Catherine Foley Geib; Hilary Chart; Mei Tan; Baruch Wolhendler; Richard K. Wagner

The literature has long pointed to heightened frequencies of learning disabilities (LD) within the population of law offenders; however, a systematic appraisal of these observations, careful estimation of these frequencies, and investigation of their correlates and causes have been lacking. Here we present data collected from all youth (1,337 unique admissions, mean age 14.81, 20.3% females) placed in detention in Connecticut (January 1, 2010–July 1, 2011). All youth completed a computerized educational screener designed to test a range of performance in reading (word and text levels) and mathematics. A subsample (n = 410) received the Wide Range Achievement Test, in addition to the educational screener. Quantitative (scale-based) and qualitative (grade-equivalence-based) indicators were then analyzed for both assessments. Results established the range of LD in this sample from 13% to 40%, averaging 24.9%. This work provides a systematic exploration of the type and severity of word and text reading and mathematics skill deficiencies among juvenile detainees and builds the foundation for subsequent efforts that may link these deficiencies to both more formal, structured, and variable definitions and classifications of LD, and to other types of disabilities (e.g., intellectual disability) and developmental disorders (e.g., ADHD) that need to be conducted in future research.


American Journal of Orthopsychiatry | 2017

Sources of Heterogeneity in Developmental Outcomes of Children With Past and Current Experiences of Institutionalization in Russia: A Four-Group Comparison.

Natalia Rakhlin; Sascha Hein; Niamh Doyle; Lesley Hart; Roman Koposov; Donna Macomber; Vladislav Ruchkin; Anastasia Strelina; Mei Tan; Elena L. Grigorenko

The present study sought to compare 4 groups of age- and gender-matched children—(a) those reared in institutions for children without parental care in Russia; (b) those raised by their biological parents in Russia; (c) those adopted to the United States from Russian institutions; and (d) those born in the United States and raised by their biological parents—on indicators of cognition, language, and early learning. In addition, we aimed to compare the effects of the length of time spent in an institution, the age of initial placement in an institution, the age at adoption, and pre-institutional risk factors (i.e., prenatal substance exposure and prematurity and low birth weight) on the above-mentioned outcomes in the 2 groups of children with institutionalization experiences. Our results confirm previous reports demonstrating negative consequences of institutionalization and substantial ameliorating effects of adoption. They also underscore the complexity of the effects of institutionalization and adoption, showing that they are intertwined with the effects of pre-institutional risk factors.


Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | 2011

Examining Specific Effects of Context on Adaptive Behavior and Achievement in Rural Africa: Six Case Studies from Southern Province, Zambia

Mei Tan; Jodi Reich; Lesley Hart; Philip E. Thuma; Elena L. Grigorenko


Journal of correctional education | 2010

Education in Juvenile Detention Facilities in the State of Connecticut: A Glance at the System.

Donna Macomber; Thomas Skiba; Jaime Blackmon; Elisa Esposito; Lesley Hart; Elisa Mambrino; Thompson Richie; Elena L. Grigorenko


Psychological Assessment | 2012

Development and Validation of the Delinquency Reduction Outcome Profile (DROP) in a Sample of Incarcerated Juveniles: A Multiconstruct/Multisituational Scoring Approach

Baptiste Barbot; Gerald J. Haeffel; Donna Macomber; Lesley Hart; John Chapman; Elena L. Grigorenko


Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | 2014

Examining the Specific Effects of Context on Adaptive Behavior and Achievement in a Rural African Community: Six Case Studies from Rural Areas of Southern Province, Zambia

Mei Tan; Jodi Reich; Lesley Hart; Philip E. Thuma; Elena L. Grigorenko

Collaboration


Dive into the Lesley Hart's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Niamh Doyle

State University of New York Upstate Medical University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge