Lin Nga
University of Southern California
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Lin Nga.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience | 2012
Nichole R. Lighthall; Michiko Sakaki; Sarinnapha Vasunilashorn; Lin Nga; Sangeetha Somayajula; Eric Y. Chen; Nicole Samii; Mara Mather
Recent research indicates gender differences in the impact of stress on decision behavior, but little is known about the brain mechanisms involved in these gender-specific stress effects. The current study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to determine whether induced stress resulted in gender-specific patterns of brain activation during a decision task involving monetary reward. Specifically, we manipulated physiological stress levels using a cold pressor task, prior to a risky decision making task. Healthy men (n = 24, 12 stressed) and women (n = 23, 11 stressed) completed the decision task after either cold pressor stress or a control task during the period of cortisol response to the cold pressor. Gender differences in behavior were present in stressed participants but not controls, such that stress led to greater reward collection and faster decision speed in males but less reward collection and slower decision speed in females. A gender-by-stress interaction was observed for the dorsal striatum and anterior insula. With cold stress, activation in these regions was increased in males but decreased in females. The findings of this study indicate that the impact of stress on reward-related decision processing differs depending on gender.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2012
Kaoru Nashiro; Michiko Sakaki; Lin Nga; Mara Mather
The ability to change an established stimulus–behavior association based on feedback is critical for adaptive social behaviors. This ability has been examined in reversal learning tasks, where participants first learn a stimulus–response association (e.g., select a particular object to get a reward) and then need to alter their response when reinforcement contingencies change. Although substantial evidence demonstrates that the OFC is a critical region for reversal learning, previous studies have not distinguished reversal learning for emotional associations from neutral associations. The current study examined whether OFC plays similar roles in emotional versus neutral reversal learning. The OFC showed greater activity during reversals of stimulus–outcome associations for negative outcomes than for neutral outcomes. Similar OFC activity was also observed during reversals involving positive outcomes. Furthermore, OFC activity is more inversely correlated with amygdala activity during negative reversals than during neutral reversals. Overall, our results indicate that the OFC is more activated by emotional than neutral reversal learning and that OFCs interactions with the amygdala are greater for negative than neutral reversal learning.
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience | 2013
Kaoru Nashiro; Michiko Sakaki; Lin Nga; Mara Mather
The ability to update associative memory is an important aspect of episodic memory and a critical skill for social adaptation. Previous research with younger adults suggests that emotional arousal alters brain mechanisms underlying memory updating; however, it is unclear whether this applies to older adults. Given that the ability to update associative information declines with age, it is important to understand how emotion modulates the brain processes underlying memory updating in older adults. The current study investigated this question using reversal learning tasks, where younger and older participants (age ranges 19–35 and 61–78, respectively) learn a stimulus–outcome association and then update their response when contingencies change. We found that younger and older adults showed similar patterns of activation in the frontopolar OFC and the amygdala during emotional reversal learning. In contrast, when reversal learning did not involve emotion, older adults showed greater parietal cortex activity than did younger adults. Thus, younger and older adults show more similarities in brain activity during memory updating involving emotional stimuli than during memory updating not involving emotional stimuli.
Cognition & Emotion | 2011
Kaoru Nashiro; Mara Mather; Marissa A. Gorlick; Lin Nga
In a typical reversal-learning experiment, one learns stimulus–outcome contingencies that then switch without warning. For instance, participants might have to repeatedly choose between two faces, one of which yields points whereas the other does not, with a reversal at some point in which face yields points. The current study examined age differences in the effects of outcome type on reversal learning. In the first experiment, the participants’ task was either to select the person who would be in a better mood or to select the person who would yield more points. Reversals in which face was the correct option occurred several times. Older adults did worse in blocks in which the correct response was to select the person who would not be angry than in blocks in which the correct response was to select the person who would smile. Younger adults did not show a difference by emotional valence. In the second study, the negative condition was switched to have the same format as the positive condition (to select who will be angry). Again, older adults did worse with negative than positive outcomes, whereas younger adults did not show a difference by emotional valence. A third experiment replicated the lack of valence effects in younger adults with a harder probabilistic reversal-learning task. In the first two experiments, older adults performed about as well as younger adults in the positive conditions but performed worse in the negative conditions. These findings suggest that negative emotional outcomes selectively impair older adults’ reversal learning.
Neuroreport | 2013
Mara Mather; Lin Nga
The thalamus plays a role in many different types of cognitive processes and is critical for communication between disparate cortical regions. Given its critical role in coordinating cognitive processes, it is important to understand how its function might be affected by aging. In the present study, we examined whether there are age differences in low-frequency fluctuations during rest in the thalamus. Across independent data sets, we found that the amplitude of low-frequency (0.01–0.10 Hz) oscillations was greater in the thalamus among older than younger adults. Breaking this low-frequency range down further revealed that this increase in amplitude with age in the thalamus was most pronounced at the low end of the frequency range (0.010–0.027 Hz), whereas in the higher low-frequency range (0.198–0.250 Hz) younger adults showed greater amplitude than older adults. These shifts in thalamic low-frequency oscillatory activity likely influence the complex dynamics of coordinated brain activity and influence cognitive performance.
Neuroreport | 2010
Mara Mather; Nichole R. Lighthall; Lin Nga; Marissa A. Gorlick
NeuroImage | 2016
Michiko Sakaki; Hyun Joo Yoo; Lin Nga; Tae-Ho Lee; Julian F. Thayer; Mara Mather
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2013
Michiko Sakaki; Lin Nga; Mara Mather
Brain Structure & Function | 2018
Hyun Joo Yoo; Julian F. Thayer; Steven G. Greening; Tae-Ho Lee; Allison Ponzio; Jungwon Min; Michiko Sakaki; Lin Nga; Mara Mather; Julian Koenig
Archive | 2017
Kaoru Nashiro; Michiko Sakaki; Lin Nga; Mara Mather