Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Louise M. Soanes is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Louise M. Soanes.


Science | 2013

Space Partitioning Without Territoriality in Gannets

Ewan D. Wakefield; Thomas W. Bodey; Stuart Bearhop; Jez Blackburn; Kendrew Colhoun; Rachel Davies; Ross G. Dwyer; Jonathan A. Green; David Grémillet; Andrew L. Jackson; Mark Jessopp; Adam Kane; Rowena H. W. Langston; Amélie Lescroël; Stuart Murray; Mélanie Le Nuz; Samantha C. Patrick; Clara Péron; Louise M. Soanes; Sarah Wanless; Stephen C. Votier; Keith C. Hamer

This Is the Place Bats, bees, seals, and many seabirds practice central-place foraging, leaving a central home site, such as a hive or a rookery, to forage in a specific territory. Such species also share the challenge of competing for local resources with individuals from separate colonies. Using satellite tags, Wakefield et al. (p. 68, published online 6 June; see the Perspective by Weimerskirch) followed over 180 northern gannets to determine potential drivers of foraging territory division. Boundaries among colonial territories arose as a result of competition with individuals from other territories. Individuals from the same colony appeared to share information about foraging sites, presumably contributing to the establishment and maintenance of specific, long-term colonial territories. Gannets establish foraging territory boundaries in colonies through local competition and information exchange. [Also see Perspective by Weimerskirch] Colonial breeding is widespread among animals. Some, such as eusocial insects, may use agonistic behavior to partition available foraging habitat into mutually exclusive territories; others, such as breeding seabirds, do not. We found that northern gannets, satellite-tracked from 12 neighboring colonies, nonetheless forage in largely mutually exclusive areas and that these colony-specific home ranges are determined by density-dependent competition. This segregation may be enhanced by individual-level public information transfer, leading to cultural evolution and divergence among colonies.


Journal of Applied Ecology | 2013

How many seabirds do we need to track to define home-range area?

Louise M. Soanes; John P. Y. Arnould; Stephen Dodd; Michael D. Sumner; Jonathan A. Green

1. In recent years, marine predator and seabird tracking studies have become ever more popular. However, they are often conducted without first considering how many individuals should be tracked and for how long they should be tracked in order to make reliable predictions of a population’s home-range area. 2. Home-range area analysis of two seabird-tracking data sets was used to define the area of active use (where birds spent 100% of their time) and the core foraging area (where birds spent 50% of their time). Analysis was conducted on the first foraging trip undertaken by the birds and then the first two, three and four foraging trips combined. Appropriate asymptotic models were applied to the data, and the calculated home-range areas were plotted as a function of an increasing number of individuals and trips included in the sample. Data were extrapolated from these models to predict the area of active use and the core foraging area of the colonies sampled. 3. Significant variability was found in the home-range area predictions made by analysis of the first foraging trip and the first four foraging trips combined. For shags, the first foraging trip predicted a 56% smaller area of active use when compared to the predictions made by combining the first four foraging trips. For kittiwakes, a 43% smaller area was predicted when comparing the first foraging trip with the four combined trips. 4. The number of individuals that would be required to predict the home range area of the colony depends greatly on the number of trips included in the analysis. This analysis predicted that 39 (confidence interval 29–73) shags and 83 (CI: 109–161) kittiwakes would be required to predict 95% of the area of active use when the first four foraging trips are included in the sample compared with 135 (CI 96–156) shags and 248 (164–484) kittiwakes when only the first trip is included in the analysis. 5. Synthesis and applications. Seabird and marine mammal tracking studies are increasingly being used to aid the designation of marine conservation zones and to predict important foraging areas. We suggest that many studies may be underestimating the size of these foraging areas and that better estimates could be made by considering both the duration and number of data logger deployments. Researchers intending to draw conclusions from tracking data should conduct a similar analysis of their data as used in this study to determine the reliability of their home-range area predictions.


Marine Biology | 2014

Factors affecting the foraging behaviour of the European shag: implications for seabird tracking studies

Louise M. Soanes; John P. Y. Arnould; Stephen Dodd; G. Milligan; Jonathan A. Green

Abstract Seabird tracking has become an ever more popular tool to aid environmental procedures such as the designation of marine protected areas and environmental impact assessments. However, samples used are usually small and little consideration is given to experimental design and sampling protocol. European shags Phalacrocorax aristotelis were tracked using GPS technology over three breeding seasons and the following foraging trip characteristics: trip duration, trip distance, maximum distance travelled from the colony, size of area used and direction travelled from colony were determined for each foraging trip. The effect of sex, year of study, breeding site, number and age of chicks and the timing of tracking on foraging behaviour were investigated using a General Estimation Equation model. A range of sampling scenarios reflecting likely field sampling were also tested to compare how foraging behaviour differed depending on composition of the sample of birds tracked. Trip distance, trip duration, maximum distance travelled and size of area used were all significantly affected by the breeding site, and the number of chicks a tracked adult was raising. The effect of sex was also seen when examining trip distance, trip duration and the maximum distance travelled. The direction travelled on a foraging trip was also significantly affected by breeding site. This study highlights the importance of sampling regime and the influence that year, sex, age, number of chicks and breeding site can have on the foraging trip characteristics for this coastal feeding seabird. Given the logistical and financial constraints in tracking large numbers of individuals, this study identifies the need for researchers to consider the composition of their study sample to ensure any identified foraging areas are as representative as possible of the whole colony’s foraging area.


Bird Conservation International | 2015

Foraging behaviour of Brown Boobies Sula leucogaster in Anguilla, Lesser Antilles: Preliminary identification of at-sea distribution using a time-in-area approach

Louise M. Soanes; Jennifer A. Bright; Mark Bolton; James Millett; Farah Mukhida; Jonathan A. Green

Seabird populations breeding in the UK Overseas Territories remain relatively understudied compared to UK seabird populations, despite their international importance. Here we present results from one of the first seabird tracking studies in the Caribbean region, of Brown Boobies Sula leucogaster breeding on the Important Bird Area (IBA) of Dog Island, Anguilla. Birds were tracked for 5–7 days during the chick-rearing period using GPS data loggers. We assess how representative the at-sea areas of use (utilisation distributions) identified from our sample of 16 birds are likely to be of those of the whole breeding colony, and examined the effect that grid cell size used in the ‘time-in-area’ analytical approach has on these predictions. We also assess the effectiveness of the BirdLife International’s seaward extension approach to marine IBA designation, where terrestrial IBAs are buffered a set distance using existing information on the foraging radii of the same or similar breeding species. Foraging trips were 125.3 ± 54.4 (SD) km long and lasted for 5.6 ± 1.95 hrs on average. Birds travelled into the waters of four neighbouring territories; Saint Martin, Saba, Saint Eustatius and Saint Barthelemy. Our models suggest that many more individuals would need to be tracked to fully identify important at-sea areas for this colony, although this depends on the scale that important areas are defined. Whilst a smaller grid cell size may be necessary for assessing fine-scale habitat use, a larger grid cell size may be more appropriate for marine spatial planning processes. Although the BirdLife Seaward extension approach using maximum foraging distance recorded from Brown Boobies at a Mexican colony predicted a smaller foraging area than that used by Dog Island birds this approach still incorporated at least 99% of their 50% UD, 98% of their 75% UD and 86% of the 95% UD.


Bird Study | 2012

Evaluation of field and analytical methods for estimating the population size of burrow-nesting seabirds from playback surveys

Louise M. Soanes; Robert J. Thomas; Mark Bolton

Capsule Acoustic playback methods have been widely used to survey burrow-nesting petrels but playback stimuli typically yield low response rates. Digitally manipulated recordings of the calls of the European Storm Petrel were created with the aim of producing an acoustic “super-stimulus” which could be used to elicit a higher response rate to improve population census methods. However, the manipulated recordings elicited a lower response than those that were unmanipulated. We also compared two methods for estimating population size of European Storm Petrels based on playback responses and found that the du Feu method is more widely applicable and precise than the standard method widely used in the UK and Ireland.


Marine Policy | 2013

Individual consistency in the foraging behaviour of Northern Gannets: Implications for interactions with offshore renewable energy developments

Louise M. Soanes; Phil W. Atkinson; R.D. Gauvain; Jonathan A. Green


Marine Policy | 2016

Important foraging areas of seabirds from Anguilla, Caribbean: Implications for marine spatial planning

Louise M. Soanes; Jennifer A. Bright; D. Carter; Maria P. Dias; T. Fleming; K. Gumbs; G. Hughes; Farah Mukhida; Jonathan A. Green


Climate Research | 2015

Effects of sea temperature and stratification changes on seabird breeding success

M. J. Carroll; Adam Butler; Ellie Owen; S. R. Ewing; T. Cole; Jonathan A. Green; Louise M. Soanes; John P. Y. Arnould; Stephen Newton; J. Baer; Francis Daunt; Sarah Wanless; Mark Newell; Gail S. Robertson; Roderick A. Mavor; Mark Bolton


Ecological Applications | 2017

Breeding density, fine-scale tracking, and large-scale modeling reveal the regional distribution of four seabird species.

Ewan D. Wakefield; Ellie Owen; Julia Baer; Matthew J. Carroll; Francis Daunt; Stephen Dodd; Jonathan A. Green; Tim Guilford; Roddy Mavor; Peter I. Miller; Mark Newell; Stephen Newton; Gail S. Robertson; Akiko Shoji; Louise M. Soanes; Stephen C. Votier; Sarah Wanless; Mark Bolton


Biological Conservation | 2016

Defining marine important bird areas: Testing the foraging radius approach

Louise M. Soanes; Jennifer A. Bright; Lauren P. Angel; John P. Y. Arnould; Mark Bolton; Maud Berlincourt; Ben Lascelles; Ellie Owen; B. Simon-Bouhet; Jonathan A. Green

Collaboration


Dive into the Louise M. Soanes's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Stephen Dodd

Royal Society for the Protection of Birds

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ellie Owen

Royal Society for the Protection of Birds

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sarah Wanless

Nature Conservancy Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Francis Daunt

Natural Environment Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

G. Milligan

University of Liverpool

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge