Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Manuel Schabus is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Manuel Schabus.


European Journal of Neuroscience | 2005

A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity.

Paul Sauseng; Wolfgang Klimesch; Waltraud Stadler; Manuel Schabus; Michael Doppelmayr; Simon Hanslmayr; Walter Gruber; Niels Birbaumer

Event‐related potentials and ongoing oscillatory electroencephalogram (EEG) activity were measured while subjects performed a cued visual spatial attention task. They were instructed to shift their attention to either the left or right visual hemifield according to a cue, which could be valid or invalid. Thereafter, a peripheral target had to be evaluated. At posterior parietal brain areas early components of the event‐related potential (P1 and N1) were higher when the cue had been valid compared with invalid. An anticipatory attention effect was found in EEG alpha magnitude at parieto‐occipital electrode sites. Starting 200 ms before target onset alpha amplitudes were significantly stronger suppressed at sites contralateral to the attended visual hemifield than ipsilateral to it. In addition, phase coupling between prefrontal and posterior parietal electrode sites was calculated. It was found that prefrontal cortex shows stronger phase coupling with posterior sites that are contralateral to the attended hemifield than ipsilateral sites. The results suggest that a shift of attention selectively modulates excitability of the contralateral posterior parietal cortex and that this posterior modulation of alpha activity is controlled by prefrontal regions.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2007

Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep

Manuel Schabus; Thien Thanh Dang-Vu; Geneviève Albouy; Evelyne Balteau; Mélanie Boly; Julie Carrier; Annabelle Darsaud; Christian Degueldre; Martin Desseilles; S. Gais; Christophe Phillips; Géraldine Rauchs; Caroline Schnakers; Virginie Sterpenich; Gilles Vandewalle; André Luxen; Pierre Maquet

In humans, some evidence suggests that there are two different types of spindles during sleep, which differ by their scalp topography and possibly some aspects of their regulation. To test for the existence of two different spindle types, we characterized the activity associated with slow (11–13 Hz) and fast (13–15 Hz) spindles, identified as discrete events during non-rapid eye movement sleep, in non-sleep-deprived human volunteers, using simultaneous electroencephalography and functional MRI. An activation pattern common to both spindle types involved the thalami, paralimbic areas (anterior cingulate and insular cortices), and superior temporal gyri. No thalamic difference was detected in the direct comparison between slow and fast spindles although some thalamic areas were preferentially activated in relation to either spindle type. Beyond the common activation pattern, the increases in cortical activity differed significantly between the two spindle types. Slow spindles were associated with increased activity in the superior frontal gyrus. In contrast, fast spindles recruited a set of cortical regions involved in sensorimotor processing, as well as the mesial frontal cortex and hippocampus. The recruitment of partially segregated cortical networks for slow and fast spindles further supports the existence of two spindle types during human non-rapid eye movement sleep, with potentially different functional significance.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2007

Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories

Steffen Gais; Geneviève Albouy; Mélanie Boly; Thien Thanh Dang-Vu; Annabelle Darsaud; Martin Desseilles; Géraldine Rauchs; Manuel Schabus; Virginie Sterpenich; Gilles Vandewalle; Pierre Maquet; Philippe Peigneux

After encoding, memory traces are initially fragile and have to be reinforced to become permanent. The initial steps of this process occur at a cellular level within minutes or hours. Besides this rapid synaptic consolidation, systems consolidation occurs within a time frame of days to years. For declarative memory, the latter is presumed to rely on an interaction between different brain regions, in particular the hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). Specifically, sleep has been proposed to provide a setting that supports such systems consolidation processes, leading to a transfer and perhaps transformation of memories. Using functional MRI, we show that postlearning sleep enhances hippocampal responses during recall of word pairs 48 h after learning, indicating intrahippocampal memory processing during sleep. At the same time, sleep induces a memory-related functional connectivity between the hippocampus and the mPFC. Six months after learning, memories activated the mPFC more strongly when they were encoded before sleep, showing that sleep leads to long-lasting changes in the representation of memories on a systems level.


Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences | 2008

Intrinsic Brain Activity in Altered States of Consciousness : How Conscious Is the Default Mode of Brain Function?

Mélanie Boly; Christophe Phillips; Luaba Tshibanda; Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse; Manuel Schabus; Thien Thanh Dang-Vu; Gustave Moonen; Roland Hustinx; Pierre Maquet; Steven Laureys

Spontaneous brain activity has recently received increasing interest in the neuroimaging community. However, the value of resting‐state studies to a better understanding of brain–behavior relationships has been challenged. That altered states of consciousness are a privileged way to study the relationships between spontaneous brain activity and behavior is proposed, and common resting‐state brain activity features observed in various states of altered consciousness are reviewed. Early positron emission tomography studies showed that states of extremely low or high brain activity are often associated with unconsciousness. However, this relationship is not absolute, and the precise link between global brain metabolism and awareness remains yet difficult to assert. In contrast, voxel‐based analyses identified a systematic impairment of associative frontoparieto–cingulate areas in altered states of consciousness, such as sleep, anesthesia, coma, vegetative state, epileptic loss of consciousness, and somnambulism. In parallel, recent functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have identified structured patterns of slow neuronal oscillations in the resting human brain. Similar coherent blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) systemwide patterns can also be found, in particular in the default‐mode network, in several states of unconsciousness, such as coma, anesthesia, and slow‐wave sleep. The latter results suggest that slow coherent spontaneous BOLD fluctuations cannot be exclusively a reflection of conscious mental activity, but may reflect default brain connectivity shaping brain areas of most likely interactions in a way that transcends levels of consciousness, and whose functional significance remains largely in the dark.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2008

Spontaneous neural activity during human slow wave sleep.

Thien Thanh Dang-Vu; Manuel Schabus; Martin Desseilles; Geneviève Albouy; Mélanie Boly; Annabelle Darsaud; Steffen Gais; Géraldine Rauchs; Virginie Sterpenich; Gilles Vandewalle; Julie Carrier; Gustave Moonen; Evelyne Balteau; Christian Degueldre; André Luxen; Christophe Phillips; Pierre Maquet

Slow wave sleep (SWS) is associated with spontaneous brain oscillations that are thought to participate in sleep homeostasis and to support the processing of information related to the experiences of the previous awake period. At the cellular level, during SWS, a slow oscillation (<1 Hz) synchronizes firing patterns in large neuronal populations and is reflected on electroencephalography (EEG) recordings as large-amplitude, low-frequency waves. By using simultaneous EEG and event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we characterized the transient changes in brain activity consistently associated with slow waves (>140 μV) and delta waves (75–140 μV) during SWS in 14 non-sleep-deprived normal human volunteers. Significant increases in activity were associated with these waves in several cortical areas, including the inferior frontal, medial prefrontal, precuneus, and posterior cingulate areas. Compared with baseline activity, slow waves are associated with significant activity in the parahippocampal gyrus, cerebellum, and brainstem, whereas delta waves are related to frontal responses. No decrease in activity was observed. This study demonstrates that SWS is not a state of brain quiescence, but rather is an active state during which brain activity is consistently synchronized to the slow oscillation in specific cerebral regions. The partial overlap between the response pattern related to SWS waves and the waking default mode network is consistent with the fascinating hypothesis that brain responses synchronized by the slow oscillation restore microwake-like activity patterns that facilitate neuronal interactions.


Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2011

Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self

Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse; Athena Demertzi; Manuel Schabus; Quentin Noirhomme; Serge Brédart; Mélanie Boly; Christophe Phillips; Andrea Soddu; André Luxen; Gustave Moonen; Steven Laureys

Evidence from functional neuroimaging studies on resting state suggests that there are two distinct anticorrelated cortical systems that mediate conscious awareness: an “extrinsic” system that encompasses lateral fronto-parietal areas and has been linked with processes of external input (external awareness), and an “intrinsic” system which encompasses mainly medial brain areas and has been associated with internal processes (internal awareness). The aim of our study was to explore the neural correlates of resting state by providing behavioral and neuroimaging data from healthy volunteers. With no a priori assumptions, we first determined behaviorally the relationship between external and internal awareness in 31 subjects. We found a significant anticorrelation between external and internal awareness with a mean switching frequency of 0.05 Hz (range: 0.01–0.1 Hz). Interestingly, this frequency is similar to BOLD fMRI slow oscillations. We then evaluated 22 healthy volunteers in an fMRI paradigm looking for brain areas where BOLD activity correlated with “internal” and “external” scores. Activation of precuneus/posterior cingulate, anterior cingulate/mesiofrontal cortices, and parahippocampal areas (“intrinsic system”) was linearly linked to intensity of internal awareness, whereas activation of lateral fronto-parietal cortices (“extrinsic system”) was linearly associated with intensity of external awareness.


Neurology | 2009

VOLUNTARY BRAIN PROCESSING IN DISORDERS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

Caroline Schnakers; Fabien Perrin; Manuel Schabus; Steve Majerus; Didier Ledoux; Pierre Damas; Mélanie Boly; Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse; Marie-Aurélie Bruno; Gustave Moonen; Steven Laureys

Background: Disentangling the vegetative state from the minimally conscious state is often difficult when relying only on behavioral observation. In this study, we explored a new active evoked-related potentials paradigm as an alternative method for the detection of voluntary brain activity. Methods: The participants were 22 right-handed patients (10 traumatic) diagnosed as being in a vegetative state (VS) (n = 8) or in a minimally conscious state (MCS) (n = 14). They were presented sequences of names containing the patient’s own name or other names, in both passive and active conditions. In the active condition, the patients were instructed to count her or his own name or to count another target name. Results: Like controls, MCS patients presented a larger P3 to the patient’s own name, in the passive and in the active conditions. Moreover, the P3 to target stimuli was higher in the active than in the passive condition, suggesting voluntary compliance to task instructions like controls. These responses were even observed in patients with low behavioral responses (e.g., visual fixation and pursuit). In contrast, no P3 differences between passive and active conditions were observed for VS patients. Conclusions: The present results suggest that active evoked-related potentials paradigms may permit detection of voluntary brain function in patients with severe brain damage who present with a disorder of consciousness, even when the patient may present with very limited to questionably any signs of awareness.


PLOS Biology | 2007

Sleep-Related Hippocampo-Cortical Interplay during Emotional Memory Recollection

Virginie Sterpenich; Geneviève Albouy; Mélanie Boly; Gilles Vandewalle; Annabelle Darsaud; Evelyne Balteau; Thien Thanh Dang-Vu; Martin Desseilles; Arnaud D'Argembeau; Steffen Gais; Géraldine Rauchs; Manuel Schabus; Christian Degueldre; André Luxen; Fabienne Collette; Pierre Maquet

Emotional events are usually better remembered than neutral ones. This effect is mediated in part by a modulation of the hippocampus by the amygdala. Sleep plays a role in the consolidation of declarative memory. We examined the impact of sleep and lack of sleep on the consolidation of emotional (negative and positive) memories at the macroscopic systems level. Using functional MRI (fMRI), we compared the neural correlates of successful recollection by humans of emotional and neutral stimuli, 72 h after encoding, with or without total sleep deprivation during the first post-encoding night. In contrast to recollection of neutral and positive stimuli, which was deteriorated by sleep deprivation, similar recollection levels were achieved for negative stimuli in both groups. Successful recollection of emotional stimuli elicited larger responses in the hippocampus and various cortical areas, including the medial prefrontal cortex, in the sleep group than in the sleep deprived group. This effect was consistent across subjects for negative items but depended linearly on individual memory performance for positive items. In addition, the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex were functionally more connected during recollection of either negative or positive than neutral items, and more so in sleeping than in sleep-deprived subjects. In the sleep-deprived group, recollection of negative items elicited larger responses in the amygdala and an occipital area than in the sleep group. In contrast, no such difference in brain responses between groups was associated with recollection of positive stimuli. The results suggest that the emotional significance of memories influences their sleep-dependent systems-level consolidation. The recruitment of hippocampo-neocortical networks during recollection is enhanced after sleep and is hindered by sleep deprivation. After sleep deprivation, recollection of negative, potentially dangerous, memories recruits an alternate amygdalo-cortical network, which would keep track of emotional information despite sleep deprivation.


European Journal of Neuroscience | 2006

Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities.

Manuel Schabus; K. Hödlmoser; Georg Gruber; Cornelia Sauter; Peter Anderer; Gerhard Klösch; Silvia Parapatics; Bernd Saletu; Wolfgang Klimesch; Josef Zeitlhofer

Stage 2 sleep spindles have been previously viewed as useful markers for the development and integrity of the CNS and were more currently linked to ‘offline re‐processing’ of implicit as well as explicit memory traces. Additionally, it had been discussed if spindles might be related to a more general learning or cognitive ability. In the present multicentre study we examined the relationship of automatically detected slow (< 13 Hz) and fast (> 13 Hz) stage 2 sleep spindles with: (i) the Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices (testing ‘general cognitive ability’); as well as (ii) the Wechsler Memory scale‐revised (evaluating memory in various subdomains). Forty‐eight healthy subjects slept three times (separated by 1 week) for a whole night in a sleep laboratory with complete polysomnographic montage. Whereas the first night only served adaptation and screening purposes, the two remaining nights were preceded either by an implicit mirror‐tracing or an explicit word‐pair association learning or (corresponding) control task. Robust relationships of slow and fast sleep spindles with both cognitive as well as memory abilities were found irrespectively of whether learning occurred before sleep. Based on the present findings we suggest that besides being involved in shaping neuronal networks after learning, sleep spindles do reflect important aspects of efficient cortical‐subcortical connectivity, and are thereby linked to cognitive‐ and memory‐related abilities alike.


Science | 2009

Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area.

Christina Schmidt; Fabienne Collette; Yves Leclercq; Virginie Sterpenich; Gilles Vandewalle; Pierre Berthomier; Christian Berthomier; Christophe Phillips; Gilberte Tinguely; Annabelle Darsaud; Steffen Gais; Manuel Schabus; Martin Desseilles; Thien Thanh Dang-Vu; Eric Salmon; Evelyne Balteau; Christian Degueldre; André Luxen; Pierre Maquet; Christian Cajochen; Philippe Peigneux

After prolonged wakefulness, people who are “night owls” do better at mental tests than those who are “early birds.” Early Birds and Night Owls In humans, peaks and troughs in alertness and cognitive performance partly depend on the timing of daily activities. Using brain imaging from the extreme ends of the spectrum of individuals whose performance is better in the mornings to those who do best in the evenings, Schmidt et al. (p. 516) found that after more than 10 hours of wakefulness, morning types had less activity in brain areas linked to attention compared with evening types. Moreover, morning types felt sleepier and tended to perform slower on psychomotor vigilance tasks. It seems getting the balance of sleep at the right time is important for regulating the daily pattern of cognitive performance. Throughout the day, cognitive performance is under the combined influence of circadian processes and homeostatic sleep pressure. Some people perform best in the morning, whereas others are more alert in the evening. These chronotypes provide a unique way to study the effects of sleep-wake regulation on the cerebral mechanisms supporting cognition. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging in extreme chronotypes, we found that maintaining attention in the evening was associated with higher activity in evening than morning chronotypes in a region of the locus coeruleus and in a suprachiasmatic area (SCA) including the circadian master clock. Activity in the SCA decreased with increasing homeostatic sleep pressure. This result shows the direct influence of the homeostatic and circadian interaction on the neural activity underpinning human behavior.

Collaboration


Dive into the Manuel Schabus's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge