Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Marc S. Tibber is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Marc S. Tibber.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2011

A common visual metric for approximate number and density

Steven C. Dakin; Marc S. Tibber; John A. Greenwood; Frederick A. A. Kingdom; Michael J. Morgan

There is considerable interest in how humans estimate the number of objects in a scene in the context of an extensive literature on how we estimate the density (i.e., spacing) of objects. Here, we show that our sense of number and our sense of density are intertwined. Presented with two patches, observers found it more difficult to spot differences in either density or numerosity when those patches were mismatched in overall size, and their errors were consistent with larger patches appearing both denser and more numerous. We propose that density is estimated using the relative response of mechanisms tuned to low and high spatial frequencies (SFs), because energy at high SFs is largely determined by the number of objects, whereas low SF energy depends more on the area occupied by elements. This measure is biased by overall stimulus size in the same way as human observers, and by estimating number using the same measure scaled by relative stimulus size, we can explain all of our results. This model is a simple, biologically plausible common metric for perceptual number and density.


Journal of Vision | 2012

Number and density discrimination rely on a common metric: Similar psychophysical effects of size, contrast, and divided attention

Marc S. Tibber; John A. Greenwood; Steven C. Dakin

While observers are adept at judging the density of elements (e.g., in a random-dot image), it has recently been proposed that they also have an independent visual sense of number. To test the independence of number and density discrimination, we examined the effects of manipulating stimulus structure (patch size, element size, contrast, and contrast-polarity) and available attentional resources on both judgments. Five observers made a series of two-alternative, forced-choice discriminations based on the relative numerosity/density of two simultaneously presented patches containing 16-1,024 Gaussian blobs. Mismatches of patch size and element size (across reference and test) led to bias and reduced sensitivity in both tasks, whereas manipulations of contrast and contrast-polarity had varied effects on observers, implying differing strategies. Nonetheless, the effects reported were consistent across density and number judgments, the only exception being when luminance cues were made available. Finally, density and number judgment were similarly impaired by attentional load in a dual-task experiment. These results are consistent with a common underlying metric to density and number judgments, with the caveat that additional cues may be exploited when they are available.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2013

Visual surround suppression in schizophrenia.

Marc S. Tibber; Elaine J. Anderson; Tracy Bobin; Elena Antonova; Alice Seabright; Bernice Wright; Patricia Carlin; Sukhwinder Shergill; Steven C. Dakin

Compared to unaffected observers patients with schizophrenia (SZ) show characteristic differences in visual perception, including a reduced susceptibility to the influence of context on judgments of contrast – a manifestation of weaker surround suppression (SS). To examine the generality of this phenomenon we measured the ability of 24 individuals with SZ to judge the luminance, contrast, orientation, and size of targets embedded in contextual surrounds that would typically influence the target’s appearance. Individuals with SZ demonstrated weaker SS compared to matched controls for stimuli defined by contrast or size, but not for those defined by luminance or orientation. As perceived luminance is thought to be regulated at the earliest stages of visual processing our findings are consistent with a suppression deficit that is predominantly cortical in origin. In addition, we propose that preserved orientation SS in SZ may reflect the sparing of broadly tuned mechanisms of suppression. We attempt to reconcile these data with findings from previous studies.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2014

A texture-processing model of the ‘visual sense of number’

Michael J. Morgan; Sabine Raphael; Marc S. Tibber; Steven C. Dakin

It has been suggested that numerosity is an elementary quality of perception, similar to colour. If so (and despite considerable investigation), its mechanism remains unknown. Here, we show that observers require on average a massive difference of approximately 40% to detect a change in the number of objects that vary irrelevantly in blur, contrast and spatial separation, and that some naive observers require even more than this. We suggest that relative numerosity is a type of texture discrimination and that a simple model computing the contrast energy at fine spatial scales in the image can perform at least as well as human observers. Like some human observers, this mechanism finds it harder to discriminate relative numerosity in two patterns with different degrees of blur, but it still outpaces the human. We propose energy discrimination as a benchmark model against which more complex models and new data can be tested.


Vision Research | 2013

Sensitivity to numerosity is not a unique visuospatial psychophysical predictor of mathematical ability.

Marc S. Tibber; Gemma S.L. Manasseh; Richard Charles Clarke; Galina Gagin; Sonja N. Swanbeck; Brian Butterworth; R. Beau Lotto; Steven C. Dakin

Highlights • Individual differences in numerosity acuity predict mathematical ability.• We tested 300+ participants to see if this relationship is unique to numerosity.• Visual numerosity and orientation task performance predicted mathematics scores.• Performance improved with age, and males significantly outperformed females.• This highlights links between mathematics and multiple visuospatial abilities.


Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science | 2014

An inability to exclude visual noise in migraine.

Marc S. Tibber; Maria Kelly; Ashok Jansari; Steven C. Dakin; Alex J. Shepherd

PURPOSE People with migraine are relatively poor at judging the direction of motion of coherently moving signal dots when interspersed with noise dots drifting in random directions, a task known as motion coherence. Although this has been taken as evidence of impoverished global pooling of motion signals, it could also arise from unreliable coding of local direction (of each dot), or an inability to segment signal from noise (noise-exclusion). The aim of this study was to determine how these putative limits contribute to impoverished motion processing in migraine. METHODS Twenty-two participants with migraine (mean age, 34.7 ± 8.3 years; 16 female) and 22 age- and sex-matched controls (mean age, 34.4 ± 6.2 years) performed a motion-coherence task and a motion-equivalent noise task, the latter quantifying local and global limits on motion processing. In addition, participants were tested on analogous equivalent noise paradigms involving judgments of orientation and size, so that the specificity of any findings (to visual dimension) could be ascertained. RESULTS Participants with migraine exhibited higher motion-coherence thresholds than controls (P = 0.01, independent t-test). However, this difference could not be attributed to deficits in either local or global processing since they performed normally on all equivalent noise tasks (P > 0.05, multivariate ANOVA). CONCLUSIONS These findings indicate that motion perception in the participants with migraine was limited by an inability to exclude visual noise. We suggest that this is a defining characteristic of visual dysfunction in migraine, a theory that has the potential to integrate a wide range of findings in the literature.


The Journal of Comparative Neurology | 2006

Cell division and cleavage orientation in the developing retina are regulated by L‐DOPA

Marc S. Tibber; Alan V. Whitmore; Glen Jeffery

Recent studies have highlighted a potential link between the cleavage orientation of a dividing neuroblast and the regulation of daughter cell fate in the developing vertebrate retina. There is evidence to suggest that this process is at least partially regulated by the presence of the retinal pigment epithelium (RPE) and/or RPE‐derived factors. In addition to a lack of melanin in the RPE, the albino retina is characterized by abnormal patterns of cell proliferation and cellular organization during development as well as cell‐type specific deficits in the adult. We examined mitotic spindle orientation in vivo in developing pigmented and albino rat retinae along with other parameters of cell division to determine whether RPE abnormalities in the albino influence these aspects of retinal development. In the albino, mitotic indices were elevated, an excess of cells remained in the cell cycle, dividing cells were not so tightly apposed to the ventricular margin, and an excessive proportion of divisions was vertically oriented (i.e., with the mitotic spindle aligned perpendicular to the plane of the neuroepithelium). Administration of L‐DOPA (a melanin precursor found at reduced concentrations in the hypopigmented eye) regulated the distribution of spindle orientations and reduced levels of mitosis in a manner consistent with an endogenous role in the control of these processes. These findings highlight the multiple roles that L‐DOPA plays in the regulation of retinal development and cast light on the diversity of anatomical abnormalities found in the albino visual system. J. Comp. Neurol. 496:369–381, 2006.


The Journal of Neuroscience | 2015

Enhanced Integration of Motion Information in Children With Autism

X Catherine Manning; Marc S. Tibber; Tony Charman; Steven C. Dakin; Elizabeth Pellicano

To judge the overall direction of a shoal of fish or a crowd of people, observers must integrate motion signals across space and time. The limits on our ability to pool motion have largely been established using the motion coherence paradigm, in which observers report the direction of coherently moving dots amid randomly moving noise dots. Poor performance by autistic individuals on this task has widely been interpreted as evidence of disrupted integrative processes. Critically, however, motion coherence thresholds are not necessarily limited only by pooling. They could also be limited by imprecision in estimating the direction of individual elements or by difficulties segregating signal from noise. Here, 33 children with autism 6–13 years of age and 33 age- and ability-matched typical children performed a more robust task reporting mean dot direction both in the presence and the absence of directional variability alongside a standard motion coherence task. Children with autism were just as sensitive to directional differences as typical children when all elements moved in the same direction (no variability). However, remarkably, children with autism were more sensitive to the average direction in the presence of directional variability, providing the first evidence of enhanced motion integration in autism. Despite this improved averaging ability, children with autism performed comparably to typical children in the motion coherence task, suggesting that their motion coherence thresholds may be limited by reduced segregation of signal from noise. Although potentially advantageous under some conditions, increased integration may lead to feelings of “sensory overload” in children with autism.


Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience | 2014

Averaging, not internal noise, limits the development of coherent motion processing

Catherine Manning; Steven C. Dakin; Marc S. Tibber; Elizabeth Pellicano

Highlights • Motion processing abilities develop gradually through childhood.• This lengthy development could be due to local noise and/or poor averaging.• 5–11-year-olds and adults performed equivalent noise and motion coherence tasks.• Through childhood, internal noise reduces and averaging increases.• Yet, only improved averaging explains developments in motion coherence sensitivity.


PLOS ONE | 2012

Reduced crowding and poor contour detection in schizophrenia are consistent with weak surround inhibition

Valentina Robol; Marc S. Tibber; Elaine J. Anderson; Tracy Bobin; Patricia Carlin; Sukhwinder Shergill; Steven C. Dakin

Background Detection of visual contours (strings of small oriented elements) is markedly poor in schizophrenia. This has previously been attributed to an inability to group local information across space into a global percept. Here, we show that this failure actually originates from a combination of poor encoding of local orientation and abnormal processing of visual context. Methods We measured the ability of observers with schizophrenia to localise contours embedded in backgrounds of differently oriented elements (either randomly oriented, near-parallel or near-perpendicular to the contour). In addition, we measured patients’ ability to process local orientation information (i.e., report the orientation of an individual element) for both isolated and crowded elements (i.e., presented with nearby distractors). Results While patients are poor at detecting contours amongst randomly oriented elements, they are proportionally less disrupted (compared to unaffected controls) when contour and surrounding elements have similar orientations (near-parallel condition). In addition, patients are poor at reporting the orientation of an individual element but, again, are less prone to interference from nearby distractors, a phenomenon known as visual crowding. Conclusions We suggest that patients’ poor performance at contour perception arises not as a consequence of an “integration deficit” but from a combination of reduced sensitivity to local orientation and abnormalities in contextual processing. We propose that this is a consequence of abnormal gain control, a phenomenon that has been implicated in orientation-selectivity as well as surround suppression.

Collaboration


Dive into the Marc S. Tibber's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Steven C. Dakin

University College London

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Geraint Rees

University College London

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Simon Grant

City University London

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Glen Jeffery

University College London

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge