Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Marcin Szczerbinski is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Marcin Szczerbinski.


Advances in Cognitive Psychology | 2009

Global and local perceptual style, field-independence, and central coherence: An attempt at concept validation.

Elizabeth Milne; Marcin Szczerbinski

Historically, the concepts of field-independence, closure flexibility, and weak central coherence have been used to denote a locally, rather globally, dominated perceptual style. To date, there has been little attempt to clarify the relationship between these constructs, or to examine the convergent validity of the various tasks purported to measure them. To address this, we administered 14 tasks that have been used to study visual perceptual styles to a group of 90 neuro-typical adults. The data were subjected to exploratory factor analysis. We found evidence for the existence of a narrowly defined weak central coherence (field-independence) factor that received loadings from only a few of the tasks used to operationalise this concept. This factor can most aptly be described as representing the ability to dis-embed a simple stimulus from a more complex array. The results suggest that future studies of perceptual styles should include tasks whose theoretical validity is empirically verified, as such validity cannot be established merely on the basis of a priori task analysis. Moreover, the use of multiple indices is required to capture the latent dimensions of perceptual styles reliably.


Journal of Child Language | 2006

Polish children's productivity with case marking: the role of regularity, type frequency, and phonological diversity*

Ewa Dabrowska; Marcin Szczerbinski

57 Polish-speaking children aged from 2;4, to 4;8 and 16 adult controls participated in a nonce-word inflection experiment testing their ability to use the genitive, dative and accusative inflections productively. Results show that this ability develops early: the majority of two-year-olds were already productive with all inflections apart from dative neuter; and the overall performance of the four-year-olds was very similar to that of adults. All age groups were more productive with inflections that apply to large and/or phonologically diverse classes, although class size and token frequency appeared to be more important for younger children (two- and three-year-olds) and phonological diversity for older children and adults. Regularity, on the other hand, was a very poor predictor of productivity. The results support usage-based models of language acquisition and are problematic for the dual mechanism model.


Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics | 2006

Investigating prosodic ability in Williams syndrome

Catherine Catterall; Sara Howard; Vesna Stojanovik; Marcin Szczerbinski; Bill Wells

This paper investigates whether people with Williams syndrome (WS) have prosodic impairments affecting their expression and comprehension of four main uses of intonation. Two adolescent males with WS were assessed using the PEPS‐C battery, which considers prosodic abilities within a psycholinguistic framework, assessing prosodic form and function in both the input and output domains. The performances of the subjects with WS were compared with control data for age and language‐comprehension matched children. The results revealed significant prosodic impairment affecting all areas of the profile. Crucially, however, different profiles of strengths and weaknesses were revealed for the two subjects. The results support the growing view that WS is a heterogeneous population in terms of linguistic abilities.


Neuropsychologia | 2007

Algebra in a man with severe aphasia

Nicolai Klessinger; Marcin Szczerbinski; Rosemary Varley

We report a dissociation between higher order mathematical ability and language in the case of a man (SO) with severe aphasia. Despite severely impaired abilities in the language domain and difficulties with processing both phonological and orthographic number words, he was able to judge the equivalence of and to transform and simplify mathematical expressions in algebraic notation. SO was sensitive to structure-dependent properties of algebraic expressions and displayed considerable capacity to retrieve algebraic facts, rules and principles, and to apply them to novel problems. He demonstrated similar capacity in solving expressions containing either solely numeric or abstract algebraic symbols (e.g., 8-(3-5)+3 versus b-(a-c)+a). The results show the retention of elementary algebra despite severe aphasia and provide evidence for the preservation of symbolic capacity in one modality and hence against the notion of aphasia as asymbolia.


Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics | 2009

Development of a test battery for assessing phonological awareness in German-speaking children

Blanca Schaefer; Silke Fricke; Marcin Szczerbinski; Annette Fox-Boyer; Joy Stackhouse; Bill Wells

The development of phonological awareness (PA), the ability to reflect on the sound structure of words independent of their meaning, has been extensively explored in English‐speaking children. However, this is not the case for other languages. The aim of this study was to develop a comprehensive PA test battery for German‐speaking preschool children, considering psycholinguistic, linguistic, and cognitive aspects and to carry out analyses of its psychometric properties. Cross‐sectional data from a sample of 55 children (CA 4;0–6;11 years) were collected. Preliminary findings confirm validity and reliability of the test battery, and support previous findings that PA develops from larger to smaller linguistic units. Phoneme‐level tasks were consistently associated with letter knowledge. The new instrument is a promising tool for basic research (e.g. cross‐linguistic comparisons of PA development) as well as for clinical and educational practice (e.g. planning speech and language therapy or literacy‐oriented intervention).


Language Acquisition | 2016

A large-scale cross-linguistic investigation of the acquisition of passive

Sharon Armon-Lotem; Ewa Hamann; Kristine M. Jensen de López; Magdalena Smoczyńska; Kazuko Yatsushiro; Marcin Szczerbinski; Anna Maria Henrica (Angeliek) van Hout; Ineta Dabasinskiene; Anna Gavarró; Erin Hobbs; Laura Kamandulytė-Merfeldienė; Napoleon Katsos; Sari Kunnari; Chrisa Nitsiou-Michaelidou; Lone Sundahl Olsen; Xavier Parramon; Uli Sauerland; Reeli Torn Leesik; Heather K. J. van der Lely

ABSTRACT This cross-linguistic study evaluates children’s understanding of passives in 11 typologically different languages: Catalan, Cypriot Greek, Danish, Dutch, English, Estonian, Finnish, German, Hebrew, Lithuanian, and Polish. The study intends to determine whether the reported gaps between the comprehension of active and passive and between short and full passive hold cross-linguistically. The present study offers two major findings. The first is the relative ease in which 5-year-old children across 11 different languages are able to comprehend short passive constructions (compared to the full passive). The second and perhaps the more intriguing finding is the variation seen across the different languages in children’s comprehension of full passive constructions. We argued, based on the present findings, that given the relevant linguistic input (e.g., flexibility in word order and experience with argument reduction), children at the age of 5 are capable of acquiring both the short passive and the full passive. Variation, however, stems from the specific characteristics of each language, and good mastery of passives by the age of 5 is not a universal, cross-linguistically valid milestone in typical language acquisition. Therefore, difficulties with passives (short or full) can be used for identifying SLI at the age of 5 only in those languages in which it has already been mastered by typically developing children.


Memory & Cognition | 2012

The role of number words: the phonological length effect in multidigit addition

Nicolai Klessinger; Marcin Szczerbinski; Rosemary Varley

We explored the role of phonological representations of number words in exact calculation. The reaction times and accuracy of responses in multidigit addition problems were compared across three groups of participants (young healthy, older healthy, and 3 patients with severe aphasia) and two types of addition problems: phonologically long in English (containing the bisyllabic number word “seven”) and short in English (monosyllabic number words—e.g., “six”). Older healthy participants were significantly faster and more accurate in calculation than younger healthy participants. The older participants showed no evidence of a phonological length effect. However this effect was apparent in the younger adults, with longer reaction times on phonologically long problems. Furthermore, there was an association between the presence of a phonological length effect and the overall speed of response, suggesting that less proficient calculators were more reliant on phonological mediation of performance. The aphasic participants retained the ability to complete multidigit additions and were as accurate as the younger healthy group, although the response times of two of the 3 patients were slow. The aphasic participants varied with regard to the presence of a phonological length effect. Two participants showed no evidence of phonological mediation, while 1 displayed a phonological length effect. The results suggest that language resources are not mandatory for exact addition, although they may be used to scaffold math performance in less competent calculators. Evidence of phonological mediation of performance in aphasic participants may provide insight into the integrity or otherwise of inner speech in severe aphasia.


Child Language Teaching and Therapy | 2011

Implementing a whole school approach to support speech, language and communication: Perceptions of key staff

Jenny Leyden; Joy Stackhouse; Marcin Szczerbinski

The role of language for learning is core across the entire school curriculum. Thus, children with speech, language and communication needs are at risk of underachieving academically. Research reports and policy drivers advocate the need for a whole school approach (WSA) to enhance children’s spoken language and communication skills, yet little is known about what it is like for schools to implement such a programme. Primary Talk is an example of a WSA, developed and piloted by the UK-based charity I CAN. This article presents an evaluation of what it was like for staff to implement a WSA in their schools. Head teachers and WSA coordinators from five schools were interviewed regarding the perceived benefits and challenges of implementing a WSA. Thematic analysis of the interview data indicated that the programme was worthwhile to implement and that it enhanced the use of visual support strategies and adult—child directed speech. The respondents also identified a number of challenges while implementing the programme relating to time constraints and maintaining the WSA as high profile in the context of competing demands in their schools.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology | 2014

The effects of age and emotional valence on recognition memory: an ex-Gaussian components analysis.

Carmen Moret-Tatay; Amparo Moreno-Cid; Irani Iracema de Lima Argimon; Tatiana Quarti Irigaray; Marcin Szczerbinski; Mike Murphy; Andrea Vázquez-Martínez; Joan Vázquez-Molina; Begoña Sáiz-Mauleón; Esperanza Navarro-Pardo; Pedro Fernández de Córdoba Castellá

The aim of this work was to study the effects of valence and age on visual image recognition memory. The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) battery was used, and response time data were analyzed using analysis of variance, as well as an ex-Gaussian fit method. Older participants were slower and more variable in their reaction times. Response times were longer for negative valence pictures, however this was statistically significant only for young participants. This suggests that negative emotional valence has a strong effect on recognition memory in young but not in old participants. The τ parameter, often related to attention in the literature, was smaller for young than old participants in an ex-Gaussian fit. Differences on the τ parameter might suggest poorer attentional performance in old participants.


Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics | 2015

The development of phonological representations in Mandarin-speaking children: Evidence from a longitudinal study of phonological awareness

Li-Li Yeh; Bill Wells; Joy Stackhouse; Marcin Szczerbinski

Abstract Two competing approaches to the analysis of the phonological structure of Mandarin syllables have been put forward. The first and more traditional approach is that a syllable can be segmented into initial consonant, medial glide, nucleus plus coda and tone. The second approach does not distinguish the non-compulsory medial glide as an independent element. To compare and evaluate these two different approaches, the development of phoneme-level awareness was investigated in 67 Mandarin-speaking children in Year 1 of school (mean age: 6;9) and Year 5 (mean age: 10;1). Results showed that at school entry some children were sensitive to glides and to a lesser extent to codas; their number increased by Year 5. This suggests that spoken language experience is enough for some children to acquire the representation of glides and codas; this is consistent with the traditional model of the Mandarin syllable, with both glides and codas as independent elements. However, the children’s task performance was generally rather poor, even in Year 5, suggesting that development of phonemic sensitivity in Mandarin speaking children is not substantially improved by increased literacy experience.

Collaboration


Dive into the Marcin Szczerbinski's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bill Wells

University of Sheffield

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Silke Fricke

University of Sheffield

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Carmen Moret-Tatay

Universidad Católica de Valencia San Vicente Mártir

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mike Murphy

University College Cork

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Irani Iracema de Lima Argimon

Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge