Maria Emília Yamamoto
Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Maria Emília Yamamoto.
International Journal of Primatology | 2000
Arrilton Araújo; Maria de Fátima Arruda; Anuska Irene Alencar; Fabíola da Silva Albuquerque; M. C. Nascimento; Maria Emília Yamamoto
Captive studies and occasional trappings of wild individuals indicate that callitrichids have small size and body weight and lack sexual dimorphism. We compared body weights of captive and wild Callithrix jacchus obtained by repeatedly weighing subjects from two populations in Brazil. We obtained captive data by routinely weighing 138 individuals from the Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte colony and wild data via regular trapping of 243 individuals in 15 free-ranging groups from IBAMAs field site in Nísia Floresta. We assigned all subjects to one of four age classes—infant, juvenile, subadult, and adult—according to their birth dates or size, reproductive status, and dental development. There is no significant difference between males and females in any of the four age classes, but captive subjects were heavier than wild ones in all age classes but infant. Reproductive and nonreproductive adult females showed no statistical difference in weight. These results accord with previous reports of lack of body size sexual dimorphism in common marmosets and suggest that differences between wild and captive common marmosets are not constitutional, but are instead a consequence of diet and physical activity. The absence of weight difference between reproductive and nonreproductive females suggests that any possible advantage from high rank is outweighed by the costs of reproduction in common marmosets.
Folia Primatologica | 2005
Maria de Fátima Arruda; Arrilton Araújo; Maria Bernardete Cordeiro de Sousa; Fabíola da Silva Albuquerque; Ana Claudia Sales da Rocha Albuquerque; Maria Emília Yamamoto
The mating system of callitrichids has been reported to be monogamous, polygynous and polyandrous. In Callithrix jacchus, groups with 2 breeding females and groups with 1 breeding female have been reported. Our purpose was to evaluate the occurrence of occasional reproduction by subordinate females in free-ranging C. jacchus groups characterized as monogamous. Four groups were monitored at a field site of IBAMA-Brazil for between 20 and 72 months. We recorded the birth of 7 sets of twins to subordinate females that had never reproduced before. Sexual activities were recorded opportunistically: dominant females copulated with only the resident male, while subordinates copulated with extra-group males. We suggest these were essentially monogamous groups that occasionally had 2 reproductive females. Between-group copulations seem to be an alternative strategy used by the subordinates. Despite the costs, there would probably be benefits as a result of scanning for vacancies for reproductive positionsin neighbouring groups and the establishment of ties with extra-group males that might become a reproductive partner in the future.
Archive | 2009
Maria Emília Yamamoto; Maria de Fátima Arruda; Anuska Irene Alencar; Maria Bernardete Cordeiro de Sousa; Arrilton Araújo
Mating systems in callitrichids are flexible, but usually only one female breeds. Reproductive skew models suggest that either dominant individuals can control the incidence of subordinate reproduction, and that when subordinates breed, it represents a concession from the dominant individual (Optimal Skew Model (OSM)), or alternatively, that subordinates breed because dominants are unable to control them (Incomplete Control Model (ICM)). We present data both from a captive study and a long term field study to test which model better explains mating systems and female-female competition in Callithrix jacchus. A 10-year study of a wild population allowed us to identify three possible mating systems occurring in groups: monogyny, monogyny with breeding attempts from subordinates (pseudo-polygyny), and polygyny. Comparison of interbirth intervals and time to produce a surviving offspring indicated that polygyny is costly to females, suggesting that females should benefit from monopolization of reproduction. Data from an experimental captive study indicated that dominant females are not always able to physiologically and/or behaviorally suppress subordinate females’ reproduction. The literature suggests that helpers are necessary to ensure the survival of dominant females’ offspring; however, our field data show that not all adults help equally. A few individuals, mostly adult males, participate significantly more in infant care than other helpers, suggesting that dominant females gain little by retaining subordinate females in their groups. These results indicate that dominant females are not able to control subordinates’ reproduction, suggesting that the flexibility of C. jacchus mating systems is better explained by ICM than by OSM.
Interdisciplinary topics in gerontology | 2008
Suzette D. Tardif; Arrilton Araújo; Maria de Fátima Arruda; Jeffrey A. French; Maria Bernardete Cordeiro de Sousa; Maria Emília Yamamoto
This chapter presents data on the relations between reproduction and aging in both captive and free-ranging marmosets and tamarins. The relationship is examined from two perspectives. First, the relation of age to physiological impairments in reproductive function is explored. Callitrichid females, in common with many other nonhuman primates, begin to display anovulation associated with follicular depletion at a point relatively close to the maximum life span. Unlike Old World primates, however, they continued to display significant steroidogenic activity in the ovary. There are age effects on some reproductive output variables, such as litter size and inter-birth interval, though the effects are often small. Like other mammals, male marmosets display a change in levels of androgens with age, although the magnitude of the decrease is not large and they actually mount an elevated response to GnRH challenge as they age. We also examined whether age affects either the establishment or maintenance of a breeding position, the factors most important in determining lifetime reproductive success. Infant mortality did increase with increasing parturitions, suggesting that there may have been aging or parturition effects on lactation. Generally, marmoset females were well past the age of sexual maturity at the beginning of the tenure and approaching 8-9 years at the end of it. Reproductive decline did not appear to be a gradual process, but a rather abrupt one, often causing the dismantling of the group. There are potentially interesting relations among maternal age, mass and declining reproductive performance, given the known importance of mass as a determinant of reproductive success in female callitrichids.
Primates | 1996
Maria Emília Yamamoto; Fabíola da Silva Albuquerque; Maria de Fátima Arruda
Data on callitrichids infant carrying is highly variable, and there is some disagreement over the relative involvement of different group members. Considering that variation may result from different social and environmental conditions it is important to compare data from different environments. The purpose of this study was to compare infant carrying in 11Callithrix jacchus groups living in two colonies, in the UK and Brazil, or in a field site in the northeastern Brazil. No differences were found in the amount of time that infants were carried, in the three environmental conditions, during weeks 1 to 4, 7, and 10. Fathers and mothers carried infants for equivalent amounts of time in the three conditions, but helpers from natural groups carried infants more than their captive counterparts. In general, adult helpers carried infants more than subadults and juveniles. The results suggest that carrying is a stable feature inCallithrix jacchus groups, but there is a trend suggestive of an extended carrying period in natural groups. Mothers behaviour were remarkably stable, but fathers behaviour, although comparable, was more conspicuous in captivity than in the field, what may be attributed to paternity certainty. Finally, the greater participation of field helpers in carrying is considered as deriving mostly to age of helpers than to group size.
International Journal of Primatology | 2004
Maria Emília Yamamoto; Fívia de Araújo Lopes
Common marmosets are omnivorous primates with a highly diversified diet. There is no study describing if and how the diet is learned. Infants get their first bits of solid food from other monkeys in the group, which suggests that they may need an introduction to food items by older individuals before including them in their diet. We assessed the acceptance of novel and familiar food items by common marmosets, both isolated and in their family groups. We tested adult, subadults and juveniles from 5 captive families while isolated and in their family groups. The test consisted of presenting for 10 min novel and familiar food items to isolated individuals or to the whole family. We recorded the latency to start eating and the number of food items ingested. When isolated, adults ate more novel and familiar food items than juveniles did. They also started eating sooner than juveniles did. When tested alone, all juveniles, except one, never tasted novel food, and juveniles ingested fewer familiar food items than adults did. When tested in their family groups, juveniles ingested more familiar and novel food than when they were isolated. Our results suggest that: 1. juvenile common marmosets show more food neophobia than adults do, especially when alone; 2. the family group may facilitate the acceptance of novel food items by juveniles; 3. the family group, besides promoting the acceptance of novel food, may also increase its ingestion; and 4. dietary acquisition in Callithrix jacchus involves social facilitation.
Psicologia: Teoria E Pesquisa | 2006
Angela Donato Oliva; Emma Otta; Fernando Leite Ribeiro; Vera Silvia Raad Bussab; Fívia de Araújo Lopes; Maria Emília Yamamoto; Maria Lucia Seidl de Moura
Biological and cultural aspects, psychological and neurological aspects, emotional and rational aspects were privileged separately in different times and perspectives - the vision of mind was sometimes focused and sometimes relegated to the black box of the psychological science. Presently, integration between mind and human behavior is sought. Mind is seen as an object of science and a product of the natural selection in the evolution of Homo sapiens. Our essay presents a brief history of the main transformations in the conception of mind and of evolutionary models of mind which contemplate reason, emotion and human actions. An effort is made to integrate and discuss the research evidence from various disciplines (anthropology, ethology, primatology, and psychology), offering an evolutionary comprehension of the human mind, of its phylogeny and ontogeny. Cooperative and competitive behavior are discussed within such framework.
Primates | 2004
Maria Emília Yamamoto; Camila Domeniconi
There is a growing body of information on sex differences in callitrichid behaviour that includes the animals’ performance in food tasks. For example, both reproductive and non-reproductive adult females have been found to be more successful than adult males in solving food tasks. In this study ten adult male and ten adult female common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), housed individually, were tested with an unfamiliar task that involved the extraction of an embedded food. The task was to open a plastic canister that contained a raisin; the open end was covered with parchment paper. Each marmoset was given 15 trials in three blocks of 5 consecutive days. We measured the latency for each animal to open the lid and get the raisin—by one of five strategies that spontaneously emerged. The females learned the task faster and more efficiently than males; all the females opened the canister on day 1, for instance, in contrast to seven of the males on the same day. Females also progressively decreased the time that they took to open the tube. The final latency on day 15, for instance, was significantly shorter for the females. These results are consistent with relevant literature for callitrichids and cannot be accounted for in terms of differences in mental abilities, strength, hand morphology, or energy requirements. Further investigation is necessary to clarify the reasons for these differences.
Behavioural Processes | 2014
Maria Emília Yamamoto; Arrilton Araújo; Maria de Fátima Arruda; Ana Karinne Moreira Lima; José de Oliveira Siqueira; Wallisen Tadashi Hattori
Marmosets are cooperative breeders organized as extended family groups, but breeding is generally restricted to a single pair. Breeding competition is fierce in female marmosets; males, on the other hand, show low levels of intragroup aggression. We investigated male and female breeding strategies and the resulting reproductive output in 9 wild groups. Reproductive output, tenure of breeding animals, identification of the breeding system, breeding position replacements, migration and infanticide were recorded; also, we recorded grooming and aggression. Replacement of the breeding male or female was observed on nine occasions. On four occasions, the son of the breeding male inherited the breeding post, but we never observed inheritance of a breeding post by a daughter. Mostly, females attained a breeding post by immigrating to a group that had a breeding vacancy. Our results showed that Callithrix jacchus males and females use different strategies to attain a breeding position and maintain it for as long as possible. These strategies prolong the tenure of the breeding position, which is the best way to produce a large number of offspring. This article is part of a Special Issue entitled: Neotropical Behaviour.
Neotropical Ichthyology | 2006
Maria do Socorro R. F. Cacho; Sathyabama Chellappa; Maria Emília Yamamoto
O acara bandeira, Pterophyllum scalaree um ciclideo nativo da Bacia Amazonica do Brasil, ee exportado como especie ornamental. Neste estudo foi investigada a importância da experiencia dos machos e do sucesso na reproducao anterior para escolha do parceiro. Para as investigacoes relativasa experiencia reprodutiva foram utilizados seis pares de machos (experientes e inexperientes) e seis femeas. Os machos foram colocados em aquarios, onde foram adicionadas as femeas (uma por aquario). A escolha do parceiro foi verificada atraves da permanencia das femeas junto a um dos machos. Para as investigacoes referentes ao sucesso reprodutivo foram testados seis pares de machos, cada par com um macho bem sucedido e outro mal sucedido na reproducao anterior, frente a uma femea bem sucedida. Foi observada a permanencia da femea com um dos machos, como indicativo de preferencia. Em seguida, cada femea foi pareada com um macho mal sucedido e avaliado o sucesso reprodutivo dos casais. As femeas preferiram machos maiores, mais agressivos, territoriais e experientes. As correlacoes entre agressividade dos machos, cuidado atraves da aeracao e sobrevivencia dos ovos foram significativas. Durante o cuidado com larvas a agressividade dos machos experientes foi significativa em relacaoa sobrevivencia delas. Alem disso, cuidado intrabucal e sobrevivencia das larvas mostraram correlacoes significativas em relacao ao cuidado realizado pelos peixes experientes, acasalados e isolados. Comparando a sobrevivencia de proles resultantes de acasalamentos com os machos experientes e inexperientes, foram observadas diferencas significativas. As correlacoes entre permanencia das femeas com machos bem sucedidos na reproducao e sobrevivencia de ovos e de larvas foram significativas. As femeas avaliam a capacidade e disposicao para investimento na criacao da prole atraves do comportamento de corte dos machos. Os machos experientes e bem sucedidos tem a preferencia das femeas e alcancam maior sucesso reprodutivo.
Collaboration
Dive into the Maria Emília Yamamoto's collaboration.
Maria Bernardete Cordeiro de Sousa
Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte
View shared research outputs