Mariëtte H. van Loon
Maastricht University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Mariëtte H. van Loon.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2017
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Anique B. H. de Bruin; Jimmie Leppink; Claudia M. Roebers
Children are often overconfident when monitoring their learning, which is harmful for effective control and learning. The current study investigated childrens (N=167, age range 7-12years) judgments of learning (JOLs) when studying difficult concepts. The main aims were (a) to investigate how JOL accuracy is affected by accessibility cues and (b) to investigate developmental changes in implementing accessibility cues in JOLs. After studying different concepts, children were asked to generate novel sentences and then to make JOLs, select concepts for restudy, and take a final test. Overconfidence for incorrect and incomplete test responses was reduced for older children in comparison with younger children. For older age groups, generating a sentence led to greater overconfidence compared with not being able to generate a sentence, which indicates that older children relied more on accessibility cues when making JOLs. This pattern differed in the youngest age group; younger children were generally overconfident regardless of whether they had generated sentences or not. Overconfidence was disadvantageous for effective control of learning for all age groups. These findings imply that instructions to encourage children to avoid metacognitive illusions need to be adapted to childrens developmental stage.
Academic Medicine | 2013
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Ellen M. Kok; Rachelle J.A. Kamp; Katerina Bohle Carbonell; Jorrick Beckers; Janneke M. Frambach; Anique B. H. de Bruin
References: 1. Cook DA, Beckman TJ. Reflections on experimental research in medical education. Adv Health Sci Educ. 2010;15:455–464. 2. Kirk RE. Experimental design. In: Millsap RE, Maydeu-Olivares A. The SAGE Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology. London, UK: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2009. 3. Issa N, Schuller M, Santacaterina S, et al. Applying multimedia design principles enhances learning in medical education. Med Educ. 2011;45:818–826. 4. Cook DA, Thompson WC, Thomas KG, Thomas MR, Pankratz VS. Impact of self-assessment questions and learning styles in Web-based learning: A randomized, controlled, crossover trial. Acad Med. 2006;81:231–238. 5. Hatala RM, Brooks LR, Norman GR. Practice makes perfect: The critical role of mixed practice in the acquisition of ECG interpretation skills. Adv Health Sci Educ. 2003;8:17–26. 6. Mamede S, van Gog T, Moura AS, et al. Reflection as a strategy to foster medical students’ acquisition of diagnostic competence. Med Educ. 2012;46:464–472. 7. Marquard JL, Henneman PL, He Z, Jo J, Fisher DL, Henneman EA. Nurses’ behaviors and visual scanning patterns may reduce patient identification errors. J Exp App. Psychol 2011;17:247–256.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2017
Patrick R. Rich; Mariëtte H. van Loon; John Dunlosky; Maria S. Zaragoza
When correcting a common misconception, it seems likely that for corrective feedback to be effective, it needs to be believed. In 2 experiments, we assessed how participants’ belief in the validity of corrective feedback regarding individual misconceptions influenced knowledge revision. After responding about the validity of a set of misconceptions, participants received either a refutation alone (feedback that they were correct or incorrect) or a refutation accompanied by a supporting explanation, and then rated their belief in the corrective feedback. One week later, participants once again responded about the validity of the misconceptions. Across both experiments, participants corrected their misconceptions more often when they believed the corrective feedback. In addition, participants corrected their misconceptions more often when they had earlier received a refutation with a supporting explanation than when they had received the refutation only. This benefit of supportive explanations on knowledge revision was mediated by belief in the feedback, suggesting that explanations enhance the effectiveness of a correction by increasing belief in the feedback. These findings imply that successful correction of common misconceptions is likely enhanced by techniques that increase people’s belief in the validity of the corrective feedback.
Learning and Instruction | 2013
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Anique B. H. de Bruin; Tamara van Gog; Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer
Contemporary Educational Psychology | 2015
Mariëtte H. van Loon; John Dunlosky; Tamara van Gog; Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer; Anique B. H. de Bruin
Acta Psychologica | 2014
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Anique B. H. de Bruin; Tamara van Gog; Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer; John Dunlosky
Metacognition and Learning | 2013
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Anique B. H. de Bruin; Tamara van Gog; Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer
Learning and Instruction | 2017
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Nesrin Destan; Manuela Spiess; Anique B. H. de Bruin; Claudia M. Roebers
Metacognition and Learning | 2017
Nesrin Destan; Manuela Spiess; Anique B. H. de Bruin; Mariëtte H. van Loon; Claudia M. Roebers
Applied Cognitive Psychology | 2017
Mariëtte H. van Loon; Claudia M. Roebers