Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Marion E. Frank is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Marion E. Frank.


Brain Research | 1990

Specificity of amiloride inhibition of hamster taste responses.

Thomas P. Hettinger; Marion E. Frank

Amiloride, a blocker of epithelial sodium channels, was found to have significant effects on electrophysiological and behavioral taste responses in the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Recordings from the whole chorda tympani nerve showed that amiloride rapidly, reversibly, and competitively inhibited responses to NaCl applied to the anterior tongue. The apparent dissociation constant for amiloride binding, extrapolated to zero NaCl concentration, was 10 nM, a value comparable to estimates for various transporting tight epithelia. Recordings from single chorda tympani nerve fibers showed that 10 microM amiloride completely inhibited responses of Na-selective N fibers but had minimal effect on responses of electrolyte-sensitive H fibers, even though both types of fibers responded well to 0.1 M NaCl. Sucrose responses were not affected by amiloride. Addition of 100 microM amiloride to 0.1 M NaCl consistently increased consumption of NaCl in two-bottle drinking tests. These data suggest that one mechanism by which the taste of NaCl is sensed, which does not require adsorption or a second messenger, involves entry of Na+ into taste bud cells through an amiloride-blockable sodium channel. Taste bud cells utilizing this mechanism exclusively activate N fibers, which are involved in the control of NaCl intake. A different mechanism for the detection of NaCl and other electrolytes is utilized by taste bud cells that activate H fibers.


Brain Research | 1987

Persistence of taste buds in denervated fungiform papillae

Mark C. Whitehead; Marion E. Frank; Thomas P. Hettinger; Lein-Tuan Hou; Hyun-Duck Nah

Taste buds in hamster fungiform papillae persist in an atrophic state for as long as 330 days after chorda tympani denervation or 50 days after combined chorda tympani-lingual nerve resection. Although taste bud structure depends on innervation, there is no absolute neural requirement for taste bud survival.


Neuroscience | 1990

Taste-responsive neurons and their locations in the solitary nucleus of the hamster

Martha McPheeters; Thomas P. Hettinger; S.C. Nuding; Lawrence D. Savoy; M.C. Whitehead; Marion E. Frank

The solitary nucleus (nucleus tractus solitarii), the first central relay for taste in mammals, was studied anatomically and physiologically in the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Activity of neurons to anterior tongue stimulation with sucrose, NaCl and KCl were extracellularly recorded. Electrolytic lesions or horseradish peroxidase deposits allowed subsequent localization of recording sites. Anterior tongue taste-responsive sites were restricted to a very small part of the rostral pole of the solitary nucleus, which is about 3% of the entire nucleus. Sites were confined to the rostral-central and rostral-lateral subdivisions of Whitehead, which contain a number of morphological cell types. Some chemotopic organization was seen with multi-unit recordings, with NaCl-selective sites concentrated rostrally and sucrose- and KCl-selective sites concentrated caudally. Sites with broad sensitivity were distributed throughout the gustatory region. Single neural units showing inhibition to taste stimuli, units highly reactive to all three stimuli, and units with high spontaneous rates were seen in the solitary nucleus, as well as units that responded very selectively and had low spontaneous rates. Single units with similar response profiles to sucrose, NaCl and KCl were not segregated to separate restricted locations within the taste-reactive region; their distributions overlapped. In the hamster, neurons in the anterior tongue taste region of the solitary nucleus process taste quality information in diverse ways. Highly reactive non-specific neurons, neurons that show inhibition, and neurons with high spontaneous rates are more frequently observed in the solitary nucleus than in the afferent input fibers of the chorda tympani nerve. The small region of the rostral pole enclosing taste-responsive neurons is complexly organized in relation to taste quality and contains a number of morphological cell types whose functional role in taste is not yet known.


Archives of Oral Biology | 1995

Effects of chlorhexidine on human taste perception.

Jill A. Helms; Mary Anne Della-Fera; April E. Mott; Marion E. Frank

Chlorhexidine gluconate at a dose used to control bacteria in the mouth has a reversible effect on taste perception. Taste-intensity ratings and taste-quality identification for concentration series of sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid and quinine hydrochloride were obtained from 15 healthy humans. The participants rinsed with 0.12% chlorhexidine for 3 min twice a day. Each individual was tested 3 times: before the 4-day rinse period, 30 min after the final rinse, and 4 days after the rinse period. Chlorhexidine rinses reduced the perceptual intensity of sodium chloride and quinine hydrochloride, not sucrose or citric acid. No effects on taste perception were detected 4 days after the rinse period. The identification of sodium chloride as salty was seriously impaired by chlorhexidine but the identification of quinine hydrochloride as bitter was not affected. Specific sites of action of chlorhexidine on the taste epithelium are not known but its effects on salty taste may be related to its strong positive charge and its effect on bitter taste may be related to its amphiphilicity. Chlorhexidine has promise as a probe of taste transduction, as well as for the management of salty/bitter dysgeusias in humans.


Physiology & Behavior | 1999

Chorda Tympani Responses in Two Inbred Strains of Mice With Different Taste Preferences

Marion E. Frank; David A. Blizard

Behavioral studies suggest that there are significant differences in the taste systems of the inbred mouse (Mus musculus) strains: C57BL/6J (B6) and DBA/2J (D2). In an attempt to understand the biological basis of the behavioral differences, we recorded whole-nerve chorda tympani responses to taste solutions and compared the results to intake of similar solutions in nondeprived mice. Stimuli included a test series composed of 0.1 M sodium chloride, 0.3 M sucrose, 10 mM sodium saccharin, 3 mM hydrochloric acid, and 3 mM quinine hydrochloride, as well as concentration series for the same substances. Neural activity of the chorda tympani that was evoked by sucrose, saccharin, or NaCl was greater in B6 than D2 mice; and neural threshold for sucrose was lower in B6 mice, but neural thresholds for HCl and quinine were lower in D2 mice. B6 mice drank more sucrose and saccharin but less quinine than D2 mice; thus, sucrose and saccharin preference were positively correlated, but NaCl and quinine aversiveness were negatively correlated with the chorda tympani results. Nonetheless, genes involved in the structuring of taste receptors and/or the chordae tympani, which transduce taste stimuli having diverse perceptual qualities, differ for the two mouse strains.


Brain Research | 1996

Responses of the hamster chorda tympani nerve to binary component taste stimuli: evidence for peripheral gustatory mixture interactions.

Bradley K. Formaker; Marion E. Frank

Studies of taste mixtures suggest that stimuli which elicit different perceptual taste qualities physiologically interact in the gustatory system and thus, are not independently processed. The present study addressed the role of the peripheral gustatory system in these physiological interactions by measuring the effects of three heterogeneous taste mixtures on responses of the chorda tympani (CT) nerve in the hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Binary taste stimuli were presented to the anterior tongue and multi-fiber neural responses were recorded from the whole CT. Stimuli consisted of a concentration series of quinine.HCl (QHCl: 1-30 mM), sodium chloride (NaCl: 10-250 mM), sucrose (50-500 mM) and binary combinations of the three different chemicals. Each mixture produced a unique pattern of results on CT response magnitudes measured 10 s into the response. Sucrose responses were inhibited by quinine in QHCl-sucrose mixtures. Neural activity did not increase when quinine was added to 50-250 mM NaCl in QHCl-NaCl mixtures. However, the neural activity elicited by sucrose-NaCl mixtures was greater than the activity elicited by either component stimulus presented alone. The results demonstrate that gustatory mixture interactions are initiated at the level of the taste bud or peripheral nerve. Mechanisms for these interactions are unknown. The results are consistent with one component stimulus modifying the interaction of the other component stimulus with its respective transduction mechanism. Alternatively, peripheral inhibitory mechanisms may come into play when appetitive and aversive stimuli are simultaneously presented to the taste receptors.


Physiology & Behavior | 2004

The distinctiveness of ionic and nonionic bitter stimuli.

Marion E. Frank; Brian P Bouverat; Bruce I. MacKinnon; Thomas P. Hettinger

The diverse chemical structures of stimuli that are bitter to humans suggest a need for multiple bitter receptors. Reactions of golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) to 1 mM quinine hydrochloride, 3 mM denatonium benzoate, 180 mM magnesium sulfate, 30-100 mM caffeine, and 1-1.5 mM sucrose octaacetate (SOA) were studied to address whether there are multiple sensations elicited by bitter stimuli. Methods included behavioral generalization of LiCl-induced conditioned taste aversions (CTAs), intake preference tests, and electrophysiological recordings from the chorda tympani (CT) nerve. The five compounds, all bitter to humans, were all innately aversive to hamsters. CTA for the ionic quinine.HCl, denatonium benzoate, and MgSO(4) mutually cross-generalized and these ionic compounds were effective CT stimuli. Yet, the hamsters were much less sensitive to denatonium than humans, requiring a 100,000 times higher concentration for detection. CTA for nonionic caffeine and SOA did not cross-generalize to quinine or the other two ionic stimuli and these nonionic compounds were not effective CT stimuli. SOA and caffeine may elicit aversive reflexes or systemic reactions rather than taste sensations in the animals. Thus, the three ionic and two nonionic compounds form separate aversive stimulus classes in hamsters, neither of which appears to be a close homologue of the human bitter taste.


Experimental Neurology | 1996

Recovery of chorda tympani nerve function following injury.

Peter Cain; Marion E. Frank; Michael A. Barry

The chorda tympani (CT) nerve carries taste information from the anterior tongue to the brain stem. Injury to the chorda tympani may result in loss or distortion of taste information. This study examined changes occurring in the hamster peripheral taste system during recovery from injury. The hamster chorda tympani nerve was crushed in the middle ear and the animals were allowed to survive from 2 to 16 weeks. At 2 weeks, CT fibers had degenerated distal to the crush site. Up to 16 weeks after crush, there were 67% fewer myelinated fibers in regenerated nerves than in controls. The mean area of the Ca(2+)-ATPase-stained core of the fungiform taste buds was significantly smaller than in controls 2 weeks after injury, but recovered to control values by 4 weeks. Electrophysiological responses to taste stimuli were recorded from the chorda tympani distal to the injury. No responses were seen after 2 weeks; weak and unstable responses were seen after 3 weeks. By 4-8 weeks, relative responses to taste stimuli were similar to control responses, but the variability of the responses to sucrose was significantly greater than that in controls. The frequency of responses to the water rinse following taste stimuli, particularly sucrose, was also greater in the regenerated nerves. The abnormal electrophysiological responses to sucrose may be the result of the differential rate of return of fiber types and/or the transduction mechanisms. In some ways, recovery of the peripheral gustatory system after damage to the chorda tympani nerve recapitulates the later stages of taste bud development.


Behavioral Neuroscience | 2003

Taste responses to mixtures: analytic processing of quality.

Marion E. Frank; Bradley K. Formaker; Thomas P. Hettinger

The tastes of 100 mM sodium chloride (NaCl), 100 mM sucrose, and 1 mM quinine hydrochloride in mixtures were investigated in golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) with a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) paradigm. CTAs, established in golden hamsters by injection of lithium chloride, were quantified as percent suppression of control 1-hr stimulus intake. CTAs for 10 of 15 stimulus pairs with common components symmetrically cross-generalized, suggesting that component qualities were recognized in binary and ternary mixtures. However, CTAs to quinine were hardly learned and were weakly expressed when quinine was mixed with NaCl, and generalizations from multiple to single stimuli were stronger than vice versa (i.e., asymmetric). The behaviors reflect peripheral inhibition and/or central mixture suppression. Nonetheless, components retain their distinct qualities in mixtures, suggesting that taste processing is analytic.


Brain Research | 1997

Opponent effects of quinine and sucrose on single fiber taste responses of the chorda tympani nerve.

Bradley K. Formaker; Bruce I. MacKinnon; Thomas P. Hettinger; Marion E. Frank

Responses of single chorda tympani fibers to mixtures of taste stimuli were studied in the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Sucrose-best neurons showed significant suppression to quinine-sucrose mixtures compared to sucrose alone. Quinine may exert its effect as an opponent stimulus in the receptor cells at the second messenger level. This suppression may make bitter quinine more readily detected when embedded in mixtures with sweeteners.

Collaboration


Dive into the Marion E. Frank's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Thomas P. Hettinger

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bradley K. Formaker

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bradley G. Rehnberg

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bruce I. MacKinnon

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David A. Blizard

Pennsylvania State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Michael A. Barry

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

April E. Mott

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Holly F. Goyert

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mark C. Whitehead

University of Connecticut Health Center

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge