Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Mark J. Edwards is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Mark J. Edwards.


Neuron | 2005

Theta Burst Stimulation of the Human Motor Cortex

Ying-Zu Huang; Mark J. Edwards; Elisabeth Rounis; Kailash P. Bhatia; John C. Rothwell

It has been 30 years since the discovery that repeated electrical stimulation of neural pathways can lead to long-term potentiation in hippocampal slices. With its relevance to processes such as learning and memory, the technique has produced a vast literature on mechanisms of synaptic plasticity in animal models. To date, the most promising method for transferring these methods to humans is repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), a noninvasive method of stimulating neural pathways in the brain of conscious subjects through the intact scalp. However, effects on synaptic plasticity reported are often weak, highly variable between individuals, and rarely last longer than 30 min. Here we describe a very rapid method of conditioning the human motor cortex using rTMS that produces a controllable, consistent, long-lasting, and powerful effect on motor cortex physiology and behavior after an application period of only 20-190 s.


The Journal of Physiology | 2008

A common polymorphism in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene (BDNF) modulates human cortical plasticity and the response to rTMS

Binith Cheeran; Penelope Talelli; Francesco Mori; Giacomo Koch; Antonio Suppa; Mark J. Edwards; Henry Houlden; Kailash P. Bhatia; Richard Greenwood; John C. Rothwell

The brain‐derived neurotrophic factor gene (BDNF) is one of many genes thought to influence synaptic plasticity in the adult brain and shows a common single nucleotide polymorphism (BDNF Val66Met) in the normal population that is associated with differences in hippocampal volume and episodic memory. It is also thought to influence possible synaptic changes in motor cortex following a simple motor learning task. Here we extend these studies by using new non‐invasive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (TDCS) techniques that directly test the excitability and plasticity of neuronal circuits in human motor cortex in subjects at rest. We investigated whether the susceptibility to TMS probes of plasticity is significantly influenced by the BDNF polymorphism. Val66Met carriers were matched with Val66Val individuals and tested on the following protocols: continuous and intermittent theta burst TMS; median nerve paired associative stimulation; and homeostatic plasticity in the TDCS/1 Hz rTMS model. The response of Met allele carriers differed significantly in all protocols compared with the response of Val66Val individuals. We suggest that this is due to the effect of BNDF on the susceptibility of synapses to undergo LTP/LTD. The circuits tested here are implicated in the pathophysiology of movement disorders such as dystonia and are being assessed as potential new targets in the treatment of stroke. Thus the polymorphism may be one factor that influences the natural response of the brain to injury and disease.


Journal of Clinical Investigation | 2008

GLUT1 mutations are a cause of paroxysmal exertion-induced dyskinesias and induce hemolytic anemia by a cation leak

Yvonne G. Weber; Alexander Storch; Thomas V. Wuttke; Knut Brockmann; Judith S. Kempfle; Snezana Maljevic; Lucia Margari; Christoph Kamm; Susanne A. Schneider; Stephan M. Huber; Arnulf Pekrun; Robert Roebling; Guiscard Seebohm; Saisudha Koka; Camelia Lang; Eduard Kraft; Dragica Blazevic; Alberto Salvo‐Vargas; Michael Fauler; Felix M. Mottaghy; Alexander Münchau; Mark J. Edwards; Anna Presicci; Francesco Margari; Thomas Gasser; Florian Lang; Kailash P. Bhatia; Frank Lehmann-Horn; Holger Lerche

Paroxysmal dyskinesias are episodic movement disorders that can be inherited or are sporadic in nature. The pathophysiology underlying these disorders remains largely unknown but may involve disrupted ion homeostasis due to defects in cell-surface channels or nutrient transporters. In this study, we describe a family with paroxysmal exertion-induced dyskinesia (PED) over 3 generations. Their PED was accompanied by epilepsy, mild developmental delay, reduced CSF glucose levels, hemolytic anemia with echinocytosis, and altered erythrocyte ion concentrations. Using a candidate gene approach, we identified a causative deletion of 4 highly conserved amino acids (Q282_S285del) in the pore region of the glucose transporter 1 (GLUT1). Functional studies in Xenopus oocytes and human erythrocytes revealed that this mutation decreased glucose transport and caused a cation leak that alters intracellular concentrations of sodium, potassium, and calcium. We screened 4 additional families, in which PED is combined with epilepsy, developmental delay, or migraine, but not with hemolysis or echinocytosis, and identified 2 additional GLUT1 mutations (A275T, G314S) that decreased glucose transport but did not affect cation permeability. Combining these data with brain imaging studies, we propose that the dyskinesias result from an exertion-induced energy deficit that may cause episodic dysfunction of the basal ganglia, and that the hemolysis with echinocytosis may result from alterations in intracellular electrolytes caused by a cation leak through mutant GLUT1.


Brain | 2012

A Bayesian account of ‘hysteria’

Mark J. Edwards; Rick A. Adams; Harriet R. Brown; Isabel Pareés; K. J. Friston

This article provides a neurobiological account of symptoms that have been called ‘hysterical’, ‘psychogenic’ or ‘medically unexplained’, which we will call functional motor and sensory symptoms. We use a neurobiologically informed model of hierarchical Bayesian inference in the brain to explain functional motor and sensory symptoms in terms of perception and action arising from inference based on prior beliefs and sensory information. This explanation exploits the key balance between prior beliefs and sensory evidence that is mediated by (body focused) attention, symptom expectations, physical and emotional experiences and beliefs about illness. Crucially, this furnishes an explanation at three different levels: (i) underlying neuromodulatory (synaptic) mechanisms; (ii) cognitive and experiential processes (attention and attribution of agency); and (iii) formal computations that underlie perceptual inference (representation of uncertainty or precision). Our explanation involves primary and secondary failures of inference; the primary failure is the (autonomous) emergence of a percept or belief that is held with undue certainty (precision) following top-down attentional modulation of synaptic gain. This belief can constitute a sensory percept (or its absence) or induce movement (or its absence). The secondary failure of inference is when the ensuing percept (and any somatosensory consequences) is falsely inferred to be a symptom to explain why its content was not predicted by the source of attentional modulation. This account accommodates several fundamental observations about functional motor and sensory symptoms, including: (i) their induction and maintenance by attention; (ii) their modification by expectation, prior experience and cultural beliefs and (iii) their involuntary and symptomatic nature.


Movement Disorders | 2007

Patients with adult-onset dystonic tremor resembling parkinsonian tremor have scans without evidence of dopaminergic deficit (SWEDDs)

Susanne A. Schneider; Mark J. Edwards; Pablo Mir; Carla Cordivari; J Hooker; John Dickson; Niall Quinn; Kailash P. Bhatia

We present the clinical details and dopamine transporter SPECT scan results of 10 patients with arm tremor, including a rest component and reduced arm swing on the affected side, in whom the possibility of PD had been raised. All patients had signs of dystonia or components of their arm tremor that were compatible with dystonic tremor, and none had true akinesia with fatiguing or decrement, even after a mean follow‐up period of 5.8 years. All patients had normal dopamine transporter SPECT scans. Clinicians should be aware that primary adult‐onset dystonia can present with an asymmetric resting arm tremor, with impaired arm swing and sometimes also facial hypomimia or a jaw tremor, but without evidence of true akinesia. Given the important consequences of misdiagnosing such patients as PD, in cases with diagnostic uncertainty functional imaging should be considered. Among patients suspected of PD, dystonic tremor may be one cause of SWEDDs (Scans Without Evidence of Dopaminergic Deficit).


Movement Disorders | 2010

Distinguishing SWEDDs Patients with Asymmetric Resting Tremor from Parkinson's Disease: A Clinical and Electrophysiological Study

Petra Schwingenschuh; Diane Ruge; Mark J. Edwards; C. Terranova; Petra Katschnig; Fátima Carrillo; Laura Silveira-Moriyama; Susanne A. Schneider; Georg Kägi; Francisco J. Palomar; Penelope Talelli; John Dickson; Andrew J. Lees; Niall Quinn; Pablo Mir; John C. Rothwell; Kailash P. Bhatia

Approximately 10% of patients diagnosed clinically with early Parkinsons disease (PD) have normal dopaminergic functional imaging (Scans Without Evidence of Dopaminergic Deficit [SWEDDs]). An important subgroup of SWEDDs are those with asymmetric rest tremor resembling parkinsonian tremor. Clinical and pathophysiological features which could help to distinguish SWEDDs from PD have not been explored. We therefore studied clinical details including non‐motor symptoms in 25 tremulous SWEDDs patients in comparison to 25 tremor‐dominant PD patients. Blinded video rating was used to compare examination findings. Electrophysiological tremor parameters and also response to a cortical plasticity protocol using paired associative stimulation (PAS) was studied in 9 patients with SWEDDs, 9 with tremor‐dominant PD (with abnormal dopamine transporter single photon emission computed tomography findings), 8 with segmental dystonia, and 8 with essential tremor (ET). Despite clinical overlap, lack of true bradykinesia, presence of dystonia, and head tremor favored a diagnosis of SWEDDs, whereas re‐emergent tremor, true fatiguing or decrement, good response to dopaminergic drugs, and presence of non‐motor symptoms favored PD. A single tremor parameter could not differentiate between groups, but the combination of re‐emergent tremor and highest tremor amplitude at rest was characteristic of PD tremor. SWEDDs and segmental dystonia patients exhibited an abnormal exaggerated response to the PAS protocol, in contrast to a subnormal response in PD and a normal response in ET. We conclude that despite clinical overlap, there are features that can help to distinguish between PD and SWEDDs which may be useful in clinical practice. The underlying pathophysiology of SWEDDs differs from PD but has similarities with primary dystonia.


Cognitive Processing | 2013

Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions

Harriet R. Brown; Rick A. Adams; Isabel Pareés; Mark J. Edwards; K. J. Friston

Active inference provides a simple and neurobiologically plausible account of how action and perception are coupled in producing (Bayes) optimal behaviour. This can be seen most easily as minimising prediction error: we can either change our predictions to explain sensory input through perception. Alternatively, we can actively change sensory input to fulfil our predictions. In active inference, this action is mediated by classical reflex arcs that minimise proprioceptive prediction error created by descending proprioceptive predictions. However, this creates a conflict between action and perception; in that, self-generated movements require predictions to override the sensory evidence that one is not actually moving. However, ignoring sensory evidence means that externally generated sensations will not be perceived. Conversely, attending to (proprioceptive and somatosensory) sensations enables the detection of externally generated events but precludes generation of actions. This conflict can be resolved by attenuating the precision of sensory evidence during movement or, equivalently, attending away from the consequences of self-made acts. We propose that this Bayes optimal withdrawal of precise sensory evidence during movement is the cause of psychophysical sensory attenuation. Furthermore, it explains the force-matching illusion and reproduces empirical results almost exactly. Finally, if attenuation is removed, the force-matching illusion disappears and false (delusional) inferences about agency emerge. This is important, given the negative correlation between sensory attenuation and delusional beliefs in normal subjects—and the reduction in the magnitude of the illusion in schizophrenia. Active inference therefore links the neuromodulatory optimisation of precision to sensory attenuation and illusory phenomena during the attribution of agency in normal subjects. It also provides a functional account of deficits in syndromes characterised by false inference and impaired movement—like schizophrenia and Parkinsonism—syndromes that implicate abnormal modulatory neurotransmission.


The Cerebellum | 2014

Non-invasive Cerebellar Stimulation—a Consensus Paper

Giuliana Grimaldi; Georgios P. Argyropoulos; A Boehringer; Pablo Celnik; Mark J. Edwards; Roberta Ferrucci; Joseph M. Galea; Stefan Jun Groiss; Koichi Hiraoka; Panagiotis Kassavetis; Elise Lesage; Mario Manto; R. C. Miall; Anna Sadnicka; Yoshikazu Ugawa; Ulf Ziemann

The field of neurostimulation of the cerebellum either with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS; single pulse or repetitive (rTMS)) or transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS; anodal or cathodal) is gaining popularity in the scientific community, in particular because these stimulation techniques are non-invasive and provide novel information on cerebellar functions. There is a consensus amongst the panel of experts that both TMS and tDCS can effectively influence cerebellar functions, not only in the motor domain, with effects on visually guided tracking tasks, motor surround inhibition, motor adaptation and learning, but also for the cognitive and affective operations handled by the cerebro-cerebellar circuits. Verbal working memory, semantic associations and predictive language processing are amongst these operations. Both TMS and tDCS modulate the connectivity between the cerebellum and the primary motor cortex, tuning cerebellar excitability. Cerebellar TMS is an effective and valuable method to evaluate the cerebello-thalamo-cortical loop functions and for the study of the pathophysiology of ataxia. In most circumstances, DCS induces a polarity-dependent site-specific modulation of cerebellar activity. Paired associative stimulation of the cerebello-dentato-thalamo-M1 pathway can induce bidirectional long-term spike-timing-dependent plasticity-like changes of corticospinal excitability. However, the panel of experts considers that several important issues still remain unresolved and require further research. In particular, the role of TMS in promoting cerebellar plasticity is not established. Moreover, the exact positioning of electrode stimulation and the duration of the after effects of tDCS remain unclear. Future studies are required to better define how DCS over particular regions of the cerebellum affects individual cerebellar symptoms, given the topographical organization of cerebellar symptoms. The long-term neural consequences of non-invasive cerebellar modulation are also unclear. Although there is an agreement that the clinical applications in cerebellar disorders are likely numerous, it is emphasized that rigorous large-scale clinical trials are missing. Further studies should be encouraged to better clarify the role of using non-invasive neurostimulation techniques over the cerebellum in motor, cognitive and psychiatric rehabilitation strategies.


Movement Disorders | 2006

Abnormalities in motor cortical plasticity differentiate manifesting and nonmanifesting DYT1 carriers

Mark J. Edwards; Ying-Zu Huang; Pablo Mir; John C. Rothwell; Kailash P. Bhatia

A mutation in the DYT1 gene causes dominantly inherited childhood‐onset primary dystonia, but intriguingly, only 30 to 40% of those who carry the mutation ever develop symptoms. We have used the unique model provided by this group of patients to investigate the hypothesis that abnormalities in brain plasticity underlie the pathophysiology of primary dystonia. We recruited 8 DYT1 gene carriers with dystonia, 6 DYT1 gene carriers without dystonia, 6 patients with sporadic primary dystonia (torticollis), and 10 healthy control subjects. Groups were age‐matched. We compared the effect in these groups of subjects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) delivered to the motor cortex, by assessing changes in corticospinal excitability following rTMS. rTMS was given in the form of theta burst stimulation (TBS) using the inhibitory protocol “cTBS” (total of 300 pulses in 50‐Hz bursts given every 5Hz). DYT1 gene carriers with dystonia and subjects with torticollis had a significantly prolonged response to rTMS in comparison with healthy subjects. In contrast, DYT1 gene carriers without dystonia had no significant response to rTMS. These data demonstrate an excessive response to an experimental “plasticity probing protocol” in subjects with dystonia, but a lack of response in genetically susceptible individuals who have not developed dystonia. These preliminary data suggest that the propensity to undergo plastic change may affect the development of symptoms in genetically susceptible individuals and that this may be an important mechanism in the pathogenesis of primary dystonia in general.


The Journal of Physiology | 2012

Cerebellar modulation of human associative plasticity.

Masashi Hamada; Gionata Strigaro; Nagako Murase; Anna Sadnicka; Joseph M. Galea; Mark J. Edwards; John C. Rothwell

Key point  •  Increases in the strength of synaptic connections in the motor cortex (long term potentiation) can be induced in humans by repetitively pairing peripheral nerve stimuli and motor cortex transcranial magnetic stimuli given 21–25 ms apart – paired associative stimulation (PAS). •  This ‘associative plasticity’ effect has been assumed to relate to synchronicity between sensory input and motor output, with a similar mechanism proposed to underlie effects at all interstimulus intervals. •  Here we show that modulation of cerebellar activity using transcranial direct current stimulation can abolish associative plasticity in the motor cortex, but only for sensory/motor stimuli paired at 25 ms, not at 21.5 ms. •  The results indicate that human associative plasticity can be affected by cerebellar activity and that at least two different mechanisms are involved in the effects previously reported in studies using PAS at different inter‐stimulus intervals.

Collaboration


Dive into the Mark J. Edwards's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Isabel Pareés

UCL Institute of Neurology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Tabish A. Saifee

UCL Institute of Neurology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anna Sadnicka

UCL Institute of Neurology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Pablo Mir

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maja Kojovic

UCL Institute of Neurology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maria Stamelou

National and Kapodistrian University of Athens

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge