Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Martien Schrooten is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Martien Schrooten.


Journal of Experimental Psychopathology | 2011

Modulation of responses to emotional information in pathological anxiety

Martien Schrooten; Fren T.Y. Smulders; Arnoud Arntz

This study demonstrates, for the first time, that in pathological anxiety, behavioural responses depend on spatial correspondence to emotional information. Anxious patients and healthy controls were presented with word pairs consisting of one emotional and one neutral word (one above the other) and were asked to respond to the print colour of each pair by pressing the upper or lower response key. Patients responded faster when the required response did spatially correspond with a threat-related or positive word than when not; controls showed no significant response modulation. Response patterns were similar with 14 ms and 500 ms word presentation. These results are taken to suggest that these anxious patients preferentially respond to threat-relevant and positive information and are impaired to counteract this early tendency.


Journal of Experimental Psychopathology | 2012

Anxiety-related biases in visual orienting and spatial motor response selection independently assessed by a probe-classification task

Martien Schrooten; Fren T.Y. Smulders; Karin Mogg; Brendan P. Bradley

This dot-probe study assessed anxiety-related biases in visual attentional orienting and spatial motor response selection (motor attention) in high- and low-trait-anxious adults, and whether anxiety-related biases depend on response speed. Emotional-neutral word pairs appeared for 14 or 500 ms, with one word of each pair replaced by a probe. Visual attention bias to emotional words was reflected by faster responses to probes replacing emotional words (spatial correspondence between probe and word positions). Response selection bias was reflected by faster responses when emotional word position (top/bottom screen) spatially corresponded to response position (upper/lower response key). Results revealed anxiety-related bias in visual attention for physical-threat words. In distributional analyses, this bias was associated with slower responses in the 14 ms condition (first task half). Results also revealed anxiety-related effects of spatial correspondence between emotional word and response, which are discussed in terms of increased bias in motor attention towards emotional stimuli in anxiety.


European Journal of Pain | 2009

963 PAIN-RELATED FEAR IS A BETTER PREDICTOR OF DISABILITY THAN IS PAIN INTENSITY, ALSO IN CHRONIC ABDOMINAL PAIN

Martien Schrooten; Madelon L. Peters; Johannes Vlaeyen

Background and Aims: Emerging literature suggests an important relationship between traumatic event histories, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and chronic pain. Currently, this relationship remains unclarified. This may be because this relationship is very complex and the ability of human beings to face and overcome traumatic experiences has been generally ignored by psychology. The present study examines self-reports of pain, traumatic event histories, PTSD symptoms, resilience, pain acceptance, procedural variables and disability in women with chronic pain. Methods: Ninety women with chronic back pain attended Primary Care services in Malaga. This sample was divided into three groups: 1) women without traumatic event histories (N=30); 2) women with traumatic event histories and low or no PTSD symptoms (N=33); and 3) women with high PTSD symptoms (N=27). During an interview, patients self-reported sociodemographic variables, pain intensity, traumatic event histories, PTSD symptoms, resilience, pain acceptance, procedural variables (fear-avoidance beliefs, catastrophizing and hypervigilance) and disability. Results: Analyses showed significant differences between the three groups in pain intensity, resilience, pain acceptance, fear-avoidance beliefs, hypervigilance and disability. Post hoc analyses showed that the third group had higher scores in pain intensity, fear-avoidance beliefs, hypervigilance and disability; moreover, this group had lower scores in pain acceptance and resilience than the other two groups. Conclusions: These results suggest that the evaluation of traumatic event histories and PTSD symptoms, and especially the ability of human beings to face traumatic experiences, may be a relevant component of a complete psychosocial assessment of chronic pain patients.


European Journal of Pain | 2009

962 ATTENTIONAL BIAS TO PAIN IS INHIBITED WHEN ONE IS PURSUING A CONCURRENT GOAL

Martien Schrooten; S. Van Damme; Geert Crombez; Madelon L. Peters; Julia Vogt; Johannes Vlaeyen

Background and Aims: Emerging literature suggests an important relationship between traumatic event histories, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and chronic pain. Currently, this relationship remains unclarified. This may be because this relationship is very complex and the ability of human beings to face and overcome traumatic experiences has been generally ignored by psychology. The present study examines self-reports of pain, traumatic event histories, PTSD symptoms, resilience, pain acceptance, procedural variables and disability in women with chronic pain. Methods: Ninety women with chronic back pain attended Primary Care services in Malaga. This sample was divided into three groups: 1) women without traumatic event histories (N=30); 2) women with traumatic event histories and low or no PTSD symptoms (N=33); and 3) women with high PTSD symptoms (N=27). During an interview, patients self-reported sociodemographic variables, pain intensity, traumatic event histories, PTSD symptoms, resilience, pain acceptance, procedural variables (fear-avoidance beliefs, catastrophizing and hypervigilance) and disability. Results: Analyses showed significant differences between the three groups in pain intensity, resilience, pain acceptance, fear-avoidance beliefs, hypervigilance and disability. Post hoc analyses showed that the third group had higher scores in pain intensity, fear-avoidance beliefs, hypervigilance and disability; moreover, this group had lower scores in pain acceptance and resilience than the other two groups. Conclusions: These results suggest that the evaluation of traumatic event histories and PTSD symptoms, and especially the ability of human beings to face traumatic experiences, may be a relevant component of a complete psychosocial assessment of chronic pain patients.


Appetite | 2008

Inducing impulsivity versus inhibition affects food intake in a sample of healthy women

Ramona Guerrieri; Chantal Nederkoorn; Martien Schrooten; Anita Jansen

Previous research has related impulsivity to increased food intake and obesity. However, the precise nature of this relationship is not clear. One possibility is that impulsivity causes overeating and hence contributes to overweight. To test this possibility, we induced impulsivity versus inhibition to see whether this would affect subsequent food intake. Participants performed the stop signal task with either an inhibition instruction or an impulsive instruction, and were then asked to do a taste test. We expected that inducing impulsivity, by training people to act impulsively during the task, would lead to more food intake during a subsequent bogus taste test compared to inducing inhibition. Restraint and current dieting were also measured. Restraint status had no effect, but dieting status turned out to be an important moderating factor: the non-dieters acted as expected and ate more after the impulsivity induction than after the inhibition induction. However, current dieters did the opposite and ate more after the inhibition induction. Possible explanations are discussed. At least for non-dieters, we can conclude that heightened impulsivity versus inhibition leads to higher food intake.


Appetite | 2009

Inducing impulsivity leads high and low restrained eaters into overeating, whereas current dieters stick to their diet

Ramona Guerrieri; Chantal Nederkoorn; Martien Schrooten; Carolien Martijn; Anita Jansen


Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry | 2007

A Simon effect for threat-related stimulus content

Martien Schrooten; Fren T.Y. Smulders


Personality and Individual Differences | 2010

Temporal dynamics of selective attention in non-clinical anxiety

Martien Schrooten; Fren T.Y. Smulders


Personality and Individual Differences | 2009

Biased processing of emotional information in sub-clinical anxiety: Evidence from Simon effects

Martien Schrooten; Fren T.Y. Smulders


Official congress program | 2010

Selective attention to pain-related information among individuals with dental complaints: the role of anticipation and fear of pain

Mirali Tabatabaei Khatibi; Martien Schrooten; E Zare; Y Ahadi Moghadam; Johannes Vlaeyen; M Dehghani

Collaboration


Dive into the Martien Schrooten's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Johannes Vlaeyen

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Arnoud Arntz

University of Amsterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge