Martin Streinzer
University of Vienna
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Martin Streinzer.
PLOS ONE | 2013
Martin Streinzer; Axel Brockmann; Narayanappa Nagaraja; Johannes Spaethe
Ranging from dwarfs to giants, the species of honeybees show remarkable differences in body size that have placed evolutionary constrains on the size of sensory organs and the brain. Colonies comprise three adult phenotypes, drones and two female castes, the reproductive queen and sterile workers. The phenotypes differ with respect to tasks and thus selection pressures which additionally constrain the shape of sensory systems. In a first step to explore the variability and interaction between species size-limitations and sex and caste-specific selection pressures in sensory and neural structures in honeybees, we compared eye size, ommatidia number and distribution of facet lens diameters in drones, queens and workers of five species (Apis andreniformis, A. florea, A. dorsata, A. mellifera, A. cerana). In these species, male and female eyes show a consistent sex-specific organization with respect to eye size and regional specialization of facet diameters. Drones possess distinctly enlarged eyes with large dorsal facets. Aside from these general patterns, we found signs of unique adaptations in eyes of A. florea and A. dorsata drones. In both species, drone eyes are disproportionately enlarged. In A. dorsata the increased eye size results from enlarged facets, a likely adaptation to crepuscular mating flights. In contrast, the relative enlargement of A. florea drone eyes results from an increase in ommatidia number, suggesting strong selection for high spatial resolution. Comparison of eye morphology and published mating flight times indicates a correlation between overall light sensitivity and species-specific mating flight times. The correlation suggests an important role of ambient light intensities in the regulation of species-specific mating flight times and the evolution of the visual system. Our study further deepens insights into visual adaptations within the genus Apis and opens up future perspectives for research to better understand the timing mechanisms and sensory physiology of mating related signals.
The Journal of Comparative Neurology | 2013
Martin Streinzer; Christina Kelber; Sarah Pfabigan; Christoph Johannes Kleineidam; Johannes Spaethe
Sexually dimorphic sensory systems are common in Hymenoptera and are considered to result from sex‐specific selection pressures. An extreme example of sensory dimorphism is found in the solitary bee tribe Eucerini. Males of long‐horned bees bear antennae that exceed body length. This study investigated the pronounced sexual dimorphism of the peripheral olfactory system and its representation in higher brain centers of the species Eucera berlandi. Eucera males have elongated antennae, with 10 times more pore plates and three times more olfactory receptor neurons than females. The male antennal lobe (AL) comprises fewer glomeruli than the female AL (∼100 vs. ∼130), of which four are male‐specific macroglomeruli. No sex differences were found in the relative volume of the mushroom bodies, a higher order neuropil essential for learning and memory in Hymenoptera. Compared with the Western honeybee, the degree of sexual dimorphism in Eucera is more pronounced at the periphery. In contrast, sex differences in glomerular numbers are higher in the eusocial honeybee and a sexual dimorphism of the relative investment in mushroom body tissue is observed only in Apis. The observed differences between the eusocial and the solitary bee species may reflect differences in male‐specific behavioral traits and associated selection pressures, which are discussed in brief. J. Comp. Neurol. 521:2742–2755, 2013.
Arthropod-plant Interactions | 2010
Martin Streinzer; T. Ellis; Hannes F. Paulus; Johannes Spaethe
Almost all species of the orchid genus Ophrys are pollinated by sexual deception. The orchids mimic the sex pheromone of receptive female insects, mainly hymenopterans, in order to attract males seeking to copulate. Most Ophrys species have achromatic flowers, but some exhibit a coloured perianth and a bright, conspicuous labellum pattern. We recently showed that the pink perianth of Ophrys heldreichii flowers increases detectability by its pollinator, males of the long-horned bee Eucera berlandi. Here we tested the hypothesis that the bright, complex labellum pattern mimics the female of the pollinator to increase attractiveness toward males. In a dual-choice test we offered E. berlandi males an O. heldreichii flower and a flower from O. dictynnae, which also exhibits a pinkish perianth but no conspicuous labellum pattern. Both flowers were housed in UV-transmitting acrylic glass boxes to exclude olfactory signals. Males significantly preferred O. heldreichii to O. dictynnae flowers. In a second experiment, we replaced the perianth of both flowers with identical artificial perianths made from pink card, so that only the labellum differed between the two flower stimuli. Males then chose between both stimuli at random, suggesting that the presence of a labellum pattern does not affect their choice. Spectral measurements revealed higher colour contrast with the background of the perianth of O. heldreichii compared to O. dictynnae, but no difference in green receptor-specific contrast or brightness. Our results show that male choice is guided by the chromatic contrast of the perianth during the initial flower approach but is not affected by the presence of a labellum pattern. Instead, we hypothesise that the labellum pattern is involved in aversive learning during post-copulatory behaviour and used by the orchid as a strategy to increase outcrossing.
Communicative & Integrative Biology | 2010
Johannes Spaethe; Martin Streinzer; Hannes F. Paulus
Sexually deceptive orchids provide no reward to their pollinators. Instead, they mimic the sex pheromone of receptive insect females to attract males which pollinate the flowers in mating attempts. Nearly all species of the Mediterranean orchid genus Ophrys are sexually deceptive and pollinated by solitary bees and wasps. Due to the use of a highly specific olfactory communication channel most Ophrys species have, in contrast to food deceptive or rewarding orchids, an inconspicuous greenish perianth and a dark brownish labellum. However, some species possess a bright pink or white perianth, and the functional significant of such colour signals in the orchid-pollinator communication system is unknown. We recently showed that the pink perianth of Ophrys heldreichii increases the performance of its bee pollinator, males of the long-horned bee Eucera (Tetralonia) berlandi, to detect the flower at short-range. At great distances (>30cm) from the flower, male search behavior was found to be olfactory guided and unaffected by the spectral property of the perianth, i.e. chromatic and green receptor-specific contrast. However, in the near vicinity of the flower (<30cm), where spatial vision is sufficient to detect the flower, search time only correlated with the green receptor-specific contrast between the perianth and the background.
Arthropod-plant Interactions | 2012
Demetra Rakosy; Martin Streinzer; Hannes F. Paulus; Johannes Spaethe
Sexually deceptive orchids mimic signals emitted by female insects in order to attract mate-searching males. Specific attraction of the targeted pollinator is achieved by sex pheromone mimicry, which constitutes the major attraction channel. In close vicinity of the flower, visual signals may enhance attraction, as was shown recently in the sexually deceptive orchid Ophrys heldreichii. Here, we conducted an in situ manipulation experiment in two populations of O. heldreichii on Crete to investigate whether the presence/absence of the conspicuous pink perianth affects reproductive success in two natural orchid populations. We estimated reproductive success of three treatment groups (with intact, removed and artificial perianth) throughout the flowering period as pollinaria removal (male reproductive success) and massulae deposition (female reproductive success). Reproductive success was significantly increased by the presence of a strong visual signal—the conspicuous perianth—in one study population, however, not in the second, most likely due to the low pollinator abundance in the latter population. This study provides further evidence that the coloured perianth in O. heldreichii is adaptive and thus adds to the olfactory signal to maximise pollinator attraction and reproductive success.
IEEE Computer | 2014
John R. Stowers; Anton L. Fuhrmann; Maximilian Hofbauer; Martin Streinzer; Axel Schmid; Michael H. Dickinson; Andrew D. Straw
Neuroscientists are using virtual reality systems, combined with other advances such as new molecular genetic tools and brain-recording technologies, to reveal how neuronal circuits process and act on visual information. The Web extra at http://youtu.be/e_BxdbNidyQ is an overview video showing the FlyVR system in operation, including four example experiments.
PLOS ONE | 2015
Kerstin Stejskal; Martin Streinzer; Adrian G. Dyer; Hannes F. Paulus; Johannes Spaethe
Mimicking female insects to attract male pollinators is an important strategy in sexually deceptive orchids of the genus Ophrys, and some species possess flowers with conspicuous labellum patterns. The function of the variation of the patterns remains unresolved, with suggestions that these enhance pollinator communication. We investigated the possible function of the labellum pattern in Ophrys heldreichii, an orchid species in which the conspicuous and complex labellum pattern contrasts with a dark background. The orchid is pollinated exclusively by males of the solitary bee, Eucera berlandi. Comparisons of labellum patterns revealed that patterns within inflorescences are more similar than those of other conspecific plants. Field observations showed that the males approach at a great speed and directly land on flowers, but after an unsuccessful copulation attempt, bees hover close and visually scan the labellum pattern for up to a minute. Learning experiments conducted with honeybees as an accessible model of bee vision demonstrated that labellum patterns of different plants can be reliably learnt; in contrast, patterns of flowers from the same inflorescence could not be discriminated. These results support the hypothesis that variable labellum patterns in O. heldreichii are involved in flower-pollinator communication which would likely help these plants to avoid geitonogamy.
bioRxiv | 2018
Martin Streinzer; Jharna Chakravorty; Johann Neumayer; Karsing Megu; Jaya Narah; Thomas Schmitt; Himender Bharti; Johannes Spaethe; Axel Brockmann
The East Himalaya is one of the world’s most biodiverse ecosystems. Yet, very little is known about the abundance and distribution of many plant and animal taxa in this region. Bumble bees are a group of cold-adapted and high altitude insects that fulfill an important ecological and economical function as pollinators of wild and agricultural flowering plants and crops. The Himalayan mountain range provides ample suitable habitats for bumble bees. Himalayan bumble bees have been studied systematically for a few decades now, with the main focus on the western region, while the eastern part of the mountain range received little attention and only a few species are genuinely reported. During a three-year survey, we collected more than 700 bumble bee specimens of 21 species in Arunachal Pradesh, the largest of the north-eastern states of India. We collected a range of species that were previously known from a very limited number of collected specimens, which highlights the unique character of the East Himalayan ecosystem. Our results are an important first step towards a future assessment of species distribution, threat and conservation. We observed clear altitudinal patterns of species diversity, which open important questions about the functional adaptations that allow bumble bees to thrive in this particularly moist region in the East Himalaya.
Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 2016
Adrian G. Dyer; Martin Streinzer; Jair E. Garcia
Please note that a previous version of this article contained a number of errors. Please see the corrected versions of Table 1, the Methods section, and the References section in the corrected version of the article. The link to it is found below.
The Journal of Experimental Biology | 2009
Martin Streinzer; Hannes F. Paulus; Johannes Spaethe