Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Micaela Fantino is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Micaela Fantino.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2011

Spatial biases in peripersonal space in sighted and blind individuals revealed by a haptic line bisection paradigm

Zaira Cattaneo; Micaela Fantino; Carla Tinti; Alvaro Pascual-Leone; Juha Silvanto; Tomaso Vecchi

Our representation of peripersonal space does not always accurately reflect the physical world. An example of this is pseudoneglect, a phenomenon in which neurologically normal individuals bisect to the left of the veridical midpoint, reflecting an overrepresentation of the left portion of space compared with the right one. Consistent biases have also been observed in the vertical and radial planes. It is an open question whether these biases depend on normal visual experience for their occurrence. Here we systematically investigated this issue by testing blindfolded sighted and early blind individuals in a haptic line bisection task. Critically, we found a robust leftward bias in all participants. In the vertical and radial planes, sighted participants showed a consistent downward and proximal bias. Conversely, the directional bias in blind participants was dependent on the final movement direction; thus, there was no general bias in either direction. These findings are discussed in terms of different reference frames adopted by sighted and blind participants when encoding space.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2010

Crossmodal interaction between the mental number line and peripersonal haptic space representation in sighted and blind individuals

Zaira Cattaneo; Micaela Fantino; Carla Tinti; Juha Silvanto; Tomaso Vecchi

Humans tend to represent numbers in the form of a mental number line. Here we show that the mental number line can modulate the representation of peripersonal haptic space in a crossmodal fashion and that this interaction is not visually mediated. Sighted and early-blind participants were asked to haptically explore rods of different lengths and to indicate midpoints of those rods. During each trial, either a small (2) or a large (8) number was presented in the auditory modality. When no numbers were presented, participants tended to bisect the rods to the left of the actual midpoint, consistent with the notion of pseudoneglect. In both groups, this bias was significantly increased by the presentation of a small number and was significantly reduced by the presentation of a large number. Hence, spatial shifts of attention induced by number processing are not limited to visual space or embodied responses but extend to haptic peripersonal space and occur crossmodally without requiring the activation of a visuospatial representation.


Acta Psychologica | 2010

Symmetry perception in the blind

Zaira Cattaneo; Micaela Fantino; Juha Silvanto; Carla Tinti; Alvaro Pascual-Leone; Tomaso Vecchi

Bilateral mirror symmetry, especially vertical symmetry, is a powerful phenomenon in spatial organization of visual shapes. However, the causes of vertical symmetry salience in visual perception are not completely clear. Here we investigated whether the perceptual salience of vertical symmetry depends on visual experience by testing a group of congenitally blind individuals in a memory task in which either horizontal or vertical symmetry was used as an incidental feature. Both blind and sighted subjects remembered more accurately configurations that were symmetrical compared to those that were not. Critically, whereas sighted subjects displayed a higher level of facilitation by vertical than horizontal symmetry, no such difference was found in the blind. This suggests that the perceptual salience of the vertical dimension is visually based.


Experimental Brain Research | 2011

Tapping effects on numerical bisection

Zaira Cattaneo; Micaela Fantino; Juha Silvanto; Giuseppe Vallar; Tomaso Vecchi

Numerical magnitude is believed to be represented along a mental number line (MNL), and there is evidence to suggest that the activation of the MNL affects the perception and representation of external space. In the present study, we investigated whether a spatial motor task affects numerical processing in the auditory modality. Blindfolded participants were presented with a numerical interval bisection task, while performing a tapping task with either their left or right hand, either in the fronto-central, fronto-left, or fronto-right peripersonal space. Results showed that tapping significantly influenced the participants’ numerical bisection, with tapping in the left side of space increasing the original tendency to err leftward, and tapping to the right reducing such bias. Importantly, the effect depended on the side of space in which the tapping activity was performed, regardless of which hand was used. Tapping with either the left or right hand in the fronto-central space did not affect the participants’ bias. These findings offer novel support for the existence of bidirectional interactions between external and internal representations of space.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2011

Blind individuals show pseudoneglect in bisecting numerical intervals

Zaira Cattaneo; Micaela Fantino; Juha Silvanto; Carla Tinti; Tomaso Vecchi

Neurologically normal individuals typically show a leftward bias—known as pseudoneglect—in bisecting physical lines as well as numerical intervals. The latter bias may reflect the spatial nature in which numbers are represented (i.e., the mental number line). In previous studies, we found that congenitally blind individuals show such leftward bias in haptic bisection. Here, we demonstrate that blind individuals also show a consistent leftward bias in numerical bisection. The leftward bias was greater when numbers were presented in descending rather than ascending order, and the magnitude of the leftward bias was comparable to that shown by a control group of blindfolded sighted participants. Our findings thus support the view that pseudoneglect operates at a mental representational level rather than being perceptually based. Moreover, the consistent leftward bias shown by blind individuals in both line and numerical bisection suggests that the right hemisphere dominance in spatial processing, resulting in an overestimation of the left side of space, develops even in the absence of any visual input.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2013

The effect of vertical and horizontal symmetry on memory for tactile patterns in late blind individuals.

Zaira Cattaneo; Tomaso Vecchi; Micaela Fantino; Andrew M. Herbert; Lotfi B. Merabet

Visual stimuli that exhibit vertical symmetry are easier to remember than stimuli symmetric along other axes, an advantage that extends to the haptic modality as well. Critically, the vertical symmetry memory advantage has not been found in early blind individuals, despite their overall superior memory, as compared with sighted individuals, and the presence of an overall advantage for identifying symmetric over asymmetric patterns. The absence of the vertical axis memory advantage in the early blind may depend on their total lack of visual experience or on the effect of prolonged visual deprivation. To disentangle this issue, in this study, we measured the ability of late blind individuals to remember tactile spatial patterns that were either vertically or horizontally symmetric or asymmetric. Late blind participants showed better memory performance for symmetric patterns. An additional advantage for the vertical axis of symmetry over the horizontal one was reported, but only for patterns presented in the frontal plane. In the horizontal plane, no difference was observed between vertical and horizontal symmetric patterns, due to the latter being recalled particularly well. These results are discussed in terms of the influence of the spatial reference frame adopted during exploration. Overall, our data suggest that prior visual experience is sufficient to drive the vertical symmetry memory advantage, at least when an external reference frame based on geocentric cues (i.e., gravity) is adopted.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2017

The Ego-Moving Metaphor of Time Relies on Visual Experience: No Representation of Time Along the Sagittal Space in the Blind

Luca Rinaldi; Tomaso Vecchi; Micaela Fantino; Lotfi B. Merabet; Zaira Cattaneo

In many cultures, humans conceptualize the past as behind the body and the future as in front. Whether this spatial mapping of time depends on visual experience is still not known. Here, we addressed this issue by testing early-blind participants in a space–time motor congruity task requiring them to classify a series of words as referring to the past or the future by moving their hand backward or forward. Sighted participants showed a preferential mapping between forward movements and future-words and backward movements and past-words. Critically, blind participants did not show any such preferential time–space mapping. Furthermore, in a questionnaire requiring participants to think about past and future events, blind participants did not appear to perceive the future as psychologically closer than the past, as it is the case of sighted individuals. These findings suggest that normal visual development is crucial for representing time along the sagittal space.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2018

The Spatial Musical Association of Response Codes does not depend on a normal visual experience: A study with early blind individuals

Zaira Cattaneo; Carlotta Lega; Luca Rinaldi; Micaela Fantino; Chiara Ferrari; Lotfi B. Merabet; Tomaso Vecchi

Converging evidence suggests that the perception of auditory pitch exhibits a characteristic spatial organization. This pitch–space association can be demonstrated experimentally by the Spatial Musical Association of Response Codes (SMARC) effect. This is characterized by faster response times when a low-positioned key is pressed in response to a low-pitched tone, and a high-positioned key is pressed in response to a high-pitched tone. To investigate whether the development of this pitch–space association is mediated by normal visual experience, we tested a group of early blind individuals on a task that required them to discriminate the timbre of different instrument sounds with varying pitch. Results revealed a comparable pattern in the SMARC effect in both blind participants and sighted controls, suggesting that the lack of prior visual experience does not prevent the development of an association between pitch height and vertical space.


Acta Psychologica | 2013

Spatial representations in blind people: the role of strategies and mobility skills.

Susanna Schmidt; Carla Tinti; Micaela Fantino; Irene C. Mammarella; Cesare Cornoldi


Neuropsychologia | 2012

Listening to numbers affects visual and haptic bisection in healthy individuals and neglect patients.

Zaira Cattaneo; Micaela Fantino; Flavia Mancini; Flavia Mattioli; Giuseppe Vallar

Collaboration


Dive into the Micaela Fantino's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Zaira Cattaneo

University of Milano-Bicocca

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lotfi B. Merabet

Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Luca Rinaldi

University of Milano-Bicocca

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Juha Silvanto

University of Westminster

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Giuseppe Vallar

University of Milano-Bicocca

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge