Michael A. Cooke
King's College London
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Michael A. Cooke.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica | 2005
Michael A. Cooke; Emmanuelle Peters; Elizabeth Kuipers; Veena Kumari
Objective: To examine the evidence for the three kinds of aetiological model that dominate the current literature on poor insight in psychosis: clinical models, the neuropsychological model, and the psychological denial model.
Schizophrenia Research | 2007
Adegboyega Sapara; Michael A. Cooke; Dominic Fannon; Alan N. Francis; Robert W. Buchanan; Anantha P. Anilkumar; Ian Barkataki; Ingrid Aasen; Elizabeth Kuipers; Veena Kumari
Previous studies have suggested a relationship between frontal lobe-based neuropsychological functions and insight in schizophrenia. There is some evidence linking both smaller whole brain volume and frontal cortical atrophy to poor insight in this population. We investigated the relationship between total as well as specific prefrontal regional volumes and insight in schizophrenia. Twenty-eight stable outpatients with schizophrenia underwent magnetic resonance imaging scanning and assessment for insight. Insight was measured using the Birchwood self-report Insight Scale and the Expanded Schedule of Assessment of Insight. The whole brain and prefrontal regional (superior frontal, middle frontal, inferior frontal and orbitofrontal) volumes were then manually measured using the Cavalieri method and established criteria. Twenty healthy subjects were also scanned to provide control data for volumetric assessments. Smaller total prefrontal grey matter volume was moderately associated with a lower level of insight into the presence of illness. At the prefrontal sub-regional level, volumes of the superior, inferior and orbitofrontal regions contributed to this relationship, especially in males. It is concluded that smaller prefrontal grey matter volume is associated with poor insight into the presence of illness in stable schizophrenia patients. Future research should examine the association of specific dimensions of insight with frontal as well as non-frontal regional brain volumes.
Schizophrenia Research | 2007
Michael A. Cooke; Emmanuelle Peters; Dominic Fannon; Anantha P. Anilkumar; Ingrid Aasen; Elizabeth Kuipers; Veena Kumari
Background The stigma and negative societal views attached to schizophrenia can make the diagnosis distressing. There is evidence that poor insight into symptoms of the disorder and need for treatment may reflect the use of denial as a coping style. However, the relationships between insight and other coping styles have seldom been investigated. Method We examined the associations between insight, distress and a number of coping styles in 65 outpatients with schizophrenia (final n = 57) in a cross-sectional study. Results We found that (i) awareness of symptoms and problems correlated with greater distress, (ii) ‘preference for positive reinterpretation and growth’ coping style correlated with lower distress and with lower symptom awareness (re-labelling), (iii) ‘preference for mental disengagement’ coping style correlated with greater distress and lower awareness of problems, and (iv) ‘social support-seeking’ coping style correlated with greater awareness of illness, but not distress. No relationship occurred between the use of ‘denial’ as a coping style and insight or distress. Conclusions Our findings demonstrate that awareness of illness and related problems is associated with greater distress in schizophrenia. However, this investigation has not supported a simple psychological denial explanation for this relationship, as complex relationships emerged between different dimensions of insight and coping styles. The negative association between ‘positive reinterpretation and growth’ and distress suggests that adopting this style may lead to re-labelling symptoms in a less distressing way. Avoidant and isolating styles of coping both appear unhelpful. Psychological interventions should aim to promote more active coping such as discussing a mental health problem with others.
Schizophrenia Research | 2008
Michael A. Cooke; Dominic Fannon; Elizabeth Kuipers; Emmanuelle Peters; Steven Williams; Veena Kumari
Background As a reflection of poor insight, people with schizophrenia often disagree with carers and clinicians about whether (a) their experiences are abnormal, (b) they are mentally ill, and (c) they need treatment. Methods This study used voxel-based morphometry to identify the associations between total and regional grey matter volumes and self-reported and observer-rated insight in 52 patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Thirty healthy participants were also studied. Results There were positive associations in patients between (i) the ability to recognise experiences as abnormal and the total and right superior temporal gyrus grey matter volumes, (ii), awareness of problems (‘something wrong’) and the left precuneus grey matter volume and (iii) awareness of symptoms and attributing them to illness and grey matter volumes in the left superior–middle temporal gyrus and the right inferior temporal and lateral parietal gyri. The ‘recognition of the need for medication’ dimension did not correlate with total or any regional grey matter volumes. Relative to controls, patients had less total and regional grey matter volumes in the thalamus and middle occipital and superior temporal gyri. Conclusions Lower grey matter volumes in the temporal and parietal regions that have been implicated in self-monitoring, working memory and access to internal mental states are associated with poor insight on certain dimensions in psychosis.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica | 2010
Emmanuelle Peters; Sabine Landau; Paul McCrone; Michael A. Cooke; Peter Fisher; Craig Steel; Ruth Evans; Ken Carswell; Katherine Dawson; Sally Williams; Alan Howard; Elizabeth Kuipers
Peters E, Landau S, McCrone P, Cooke M, Fisher P, Steel C, Evans R, Carswell K, Dawson K, Williams S, Howard A, Kuipers E. A randomised controlled trial of cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis in a routine clinical service.
European Psychiatry | 2008
Preethi Premkumar; Michael A. Cooke; Dominic Fannon; Emmanuelle Peters; Tanja M. Michel; Ingrid Aasen; Robin M. Murray; Elizabeth Kuipers; Veena Kumari
Background While it is known that patients with schizophrenia recognize facial emotions, specifically negative emotions, less accurately, little is known about how they misattribute these emotions to other emotions and whether such misattribution biases are associated with symptoms, course of the disorder, or certain cognitive functions. Method Outpatients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (n = 73) and healthy controls (n = 30) performed a computerised Facial Emotion Attribution Test and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Patients were also rated on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). Results Patients were poor at recognizing fearful and angry emotions and attributed fear to angry and angry to neutral expressions. Fear-as-anger misattributions were predicted independently by a longer duration of illness and WCST perseverative errors. Conclusion The findings show a bias towards misattributing fearful and angry facial emotions. The propensity for fear-as-anger misattribution biases increases as the length of time that the disorder is experienced increases and a more rigid style of information processing is used. This, at least in part, may be perpetuated by subtle fearfulness expressed by others while interacting with people with schizophrenia.
Schizophrenia Bulletin | 2010
Veena Kumari; Dominic Fannon; Dominic H. ffytche; Vinodkumar Raveendran; Elena Antonova; Preethi Premkumar; Michael A. Cooke; Ananatha P.P. Anilkumar; Steven Williams; Christopher Andrew; Louise Johns; Cynthia H.Y. Fu; Philip McGuire; Elizabeth Kuipers
Previous small-sample studies have shown altered frontotemporal activity in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations and impaired monitoring of self-generated speech. We examined a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia (n = 63) and a representative group of healthy controls (n = 20) to disentangle performance, illness, and symptom-related effects in functional magnetic resonance imaging–detected brain abnormalities during monitoring of self- and externally generated speech in schizophrenia. Our results revealed activation of the thalamus (medial geniculate nucleus, MGN) and frontotemporal regions with accurate monitoring across all participants. Less activation of the thalamus (MGN, pulvinar) and superior-middle temporal and inferior frontal gyri occurred in poorly performing patients (1 standard deviation below controls’ mean; n = 36), relative to the combined group of controls and well-performing patients. In patients, (1) greater deactivation of the ventral striatum and hypothalamus to own voice, combined with nonsignificant activation of the same regions to others’ voice, associated positively with negative symptoms (blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive social avoidance) regardless of performance and (2) exaggerated activation of the right superior-middle temporal gyrus during undistorted, relative to distorted, feedback associated with both positive symptoms (hallucinations, persecution) and poor performance. A further thalamic abnormality characterized schizophrenia patients regardless of performance and symptoms. We conclude that hypoactivation of a neural network comprised of the thalamus and frontotemporal regions underlies impaired speech monitoring in schizophrenia. Positive symptoms and poor monitoring share a common activation abnormality in the right superior temporal gyrus during processing of degraded speech. Altered striatal and hypothalamic modulation to own and others’ voice characterizes emotionally withdrawn and socially avoidant patients.
Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2010
Michael A. Cooke; Emmanuelle Peters; Dominic Fannon; Ingrid Aasen; Elizabeth Kuipers; Veena Kumari
Cognitive insight in schizophrenia encompasses the evaluation and reinterpretation of distorted beliefs and appraisals. We investigated the neuropsychological basis of cognitive insight in psychosis. It was predicted that, like clinical insight, cognitive insight would be associated with a wide range of neuropsychological functions, but would be most strongly associated with measures of executive function. Sixty-five outpatients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were assessed on tests of intelligence quotient (IQ), executive function, verbal fluency, attention and memory, and completed the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale, which includes two subscales, self-certainty and self-reflection. Higher self-certainty scores reflect greater certainty about being right and more resistant to correction (poor insight), while higher self-reflection scores indicate the expression of introspection and the willingness to acknowledge fallibility (good insight). The self-certainty dimension of poor cognitive insight was significantly associated with lower scores on the Behavioural Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome; this relationship was not mediated by IQ. There were no relationships between self-reflection and any neuropsychological measures. We conclude that greater self-certainty (poor cognitive insight) is modestly associated with poorer executive function in psychotic individuals; self-reflection has no association with executive function. The self-certainty and self-reflection dimensions of cognitive insight have differential correlates, and probably different mechanisms, in psychosis.
Behavioural Brain Research | 2008
Preethi Premkumar; Dominic Fannon; Elizabeth Kuipers; Michael A. Cooke; Andrew Simmons; Veena Kumari
The frontal lobe has an extended maturation period and may be vulnerable to the long-term effects of schizophrenia. We tested this hypothesis by studying the relationship between duration of illness (DoI), grey matter (GM) and cerebro-spinal fluid (CSF) volume across the whole brain. Sixty-four patients with schizophrenia and 25 healthy controls underwent structural MRI scanning and neuropsychological assessment. We performed regression analyses in patients to examine the relationship between DoI and GM and CSF volumes across the whole brain, and correlations in controls between age and GM or CSF volume of the regions where GM or CSF volumes were associated with DoI in patients. Correlations were also performed between GM volume in the regions associated with DoI and neuropsychological performance. A longer DoI was associated with lower GM volume in the left dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (PFC), right middle frontal cortex, left fusiform gyrus (FG) and left cerebellum (lobule III). Additionally, age was inversely associated with GM volume in the left dorsomedial PFC in patients, and in the left FG and CSF excess near the left cerebellum in healthy controls. Greater GM volume in the left dorsomedial PFC was associated with better working memory, attention and psychomotor speed in patients. Our findings suggest that the right middle frontal cortex is particularly vulnerable to the long-term effect of schizophrenia illness whereas the dorsomedial PFC, FG and cerebellum are affected by both a long DoI and aging. The effect of illness chronicity on GM volume in the left dorsomedial PFC may be extended to brain structure-neuropsychological function relationships.
Psychological Medicine | 2012
Emmanuelle Peters; Steven Williams; Michael A. Cooke; Elizabeth Kuipers
BACKGROUND Previous studies have suggested that beliefs about voices mediate the relationship between actual voice experience and behavioural and affective response. METHOD We investigated beliefs about voice power (omnipotence), voice intent (malevolence/benevolence) and emotional and behavioural response (resistance/engagement) using the Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire - Revised (BAVQ-R) in 46 voice hearers. Distress was assessed using a wide range of measures: voice-related distress, depression, anxiety, self-esteem and suicidal ideation. Voice topography was assessed using measures of voice severity, frequency and intensity. We predicted that beliefs about voices would show a stronger association with distress than voice topography. RESULTS Omnipotence had the strongest associations with all measures of distress included in the study whereas malevolence was related to resistance, and benevolence to engagement. As predicted, voice severity, frequency and intensity were not related to distress once beliefs were accounted for. CONCLUSIONS These results concur with previous findings that beliefs about voice power are key determinants of distress in voice hearers, and should be targeted specifically in psychological interventions.