Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Michael J. Cahill is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Michael J. Cahill.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2014

Individual differences in learning and transfer: stable tendencies for learning exemplars versus abstracting rules.

Mark A. McDaniel; Michael J. Cahill; Mathew Robbins; Chelsea Wiener

We hypothesize that during training some learners may focus on acquiring the particular exemplars and responses associated with the exemplars (termed exemplar learners), whereas other learners attempt to abstract underlying regularities reflected in the particular exemplars linked to an appropriate response (termed rule learners). Supporting this distinction, after training (on a function-learning task), participants displayed an extrapolation profile reflecting either acquisition of the trained cue-criterion associations (exemplar learners) or abstraction of the function rule (rule learners; Studies 1a and 1b). Further, working memory capacity (measured by operation span [Ospan]) was associated with the tendency to rely on rule versus exemplar processes. Studies 1c and 2 examined the persistence of these learning tendencies on several categorization tasks. Study 1c showed that rule learners were more likely than exemplar learners (indexed a priori by extrapolation profiles) to resist using idiosyncratic features (exemplar similarity) in generalization (transfer) of the trained category. Study 2 showed that the rule learners but not the exemplar learners performed well on a novel categorization task (transfer) after training on an abstract coherent category. These patterns suggest that in complex conceptual tasks, (a) individuals tend to either focus on exemplars during learning or on extracting some abstraction of the concept, (b) this tendency might be a relatively stable characteristic of the individual, and (c) transfer patterns are determined by that tendency.


Memory & Cognition | 2011

Dissociative effects of orthographic distinctiveness in pure and mixed lists: an item-order account

Mark A. McDaniel; Michael J. Cahill; Julie M. Bugg; Nathaniel G. Meadow

We apply the item-order theory of list composition effects in free recall to the orthographic distinctiveness effect. The item-order account assumes that orthographically distinct items advantage item-specific encoding in both mixed and pure lists, but at the expense of exploiting relational information present in the list. Experiment 1 replicated the typical free recall advantage of orthographically distinct items in mixed lists and the elimination of that advantage in pure lists. Supporting the item-order account, recognition performances indicated that orthographically distinct items received greater item-specific encoding than did orthographically common items in mixed and pure lists (Experiments 1 and 2). Furthermore, order memory (input–output correspondence and sequential contiguity effects) was evident in recall of pure unstructured common lists, but not in recall of unstructured distinct lists (Experiment 1). These combined patterns, although not anticipated by prevailing views, are consistent with an item-order account.


Self and Identity | 2006

Two paths of defense: Specific versus compensatory reactions to self-threat

Donna Eisenstadt; John L. Hicks; Kevin P. McIntyre; Jennifer A. Rivers; Michael J. Cahill

Eisenstadt and Leippe (1994) and Eisenstadt, Leippe, and Rivers (2002) proposed that people respond to threatening feedback by engaging in a self-comparison process in which they ultimately accept or reject the feedback based on its refutability. The aims of the current study were to test the processes involved in: (a) specific feedback acceptance and refutation; and (b) compensatory adjustments in the self-concept following feedback. Participants received threatening feedback of high or low importance while under high or low cognitive load and then rated the self-descriptiveness of the specific feedback trait, non-feedback traits, affect, state self-esteem, and listed thoughts about the feedback. As hypothesized, counterarguments mediated acceptance of the specific feedback. Affect and state self-esteem, however, mediated compensatory adjustments in the self-concept.


Consciousness and Cognition | 2013

Resource depletion does not influence prospective memory in college students

Jill Talley Shelton; Michael J. Cahill; Hillary G. Mullet; Michael K. Scullin; Gilles O. Einstein; Mark A. McDaniel

This paper reports an experiment designed to investigate the potential influence of prior acts of self-control on subsequent prospective memory performance. College undergraduates (n=146) performed either a cognitively depleting initial task (e.g., mostly incongruent Stroop task) or a less resource-consuming version of that task (e.g., all congruent Stroop task). Subsequently, participants completed a prospective memory task that required attentionally demanding monitoring processes. The results demonstrated that prior acts of self-control do not impair the ability to execute a future intention in college-aged adults. We conceptually replicated these results in three additional depletion and prospective memory experiments. This research extends a growing number of studies demonstrating the boundary conditions of the resource depletion effect in cognitive tasks.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2016

The curious case of orthographic distinctiveness: Disruption of categorical processing.

Mark A. McDaniel; Michael J. Cahill; Julie M. Bugg

How does orthographic distinctiveness affect recall of structured (categorized) word lists? On one theory, enhanced item-specific information (e.g., more distinct encoding) in concert with robust relational information (e.g., categorical information) optimally supports free recall. This predicts that for categorically structured lists, orthographically distinct (OD) word lists should be recalled better than orthographically common (OC) word lists. Another possibility is that OD items produce a far-reaching impairment in relational processing, including that of categorical information. This view anticipates an advantage in recall for OC items relative to OD lists. In Experiment 1 categorically structured OC lists produced better recall performance and higher clustering than did categorically structured OD lists. When words were presented in capital letters, thereby minimizing orthographic distinctiveness, OC and OD lists showed equivalent recall and category clustering (Experiment 2). When recall was cued with category labels, OC items were still better recalled than OD items (Experiment 3). These patterns, along with category access and items-per-category recalled, are consistent with the interpretation that orthographic distinctiveness creates a disruption in encoding of inter-item associations within a category. This interpretation expands previous work indicating that orthographic distinctiveness disrupts encoding of serial order information, another kind of inter-item association. (PsycINFO Database Record


Chemistry Education Research and Practice | 2018

Improving general chemistry performance through a growth mindset intervention: selective effects on underrepresented minorities

Angela Fink; Michael J. Cahill; Mark A. McDaniel; Arielle Hoffman; Regina F. Frey

Women and minorities remain underrepresented in chemistry bachelors degree attainment in the United States, despite efforts to improve their early chemistry achievement through supplemental academic programs and active-learning approaches. We propose an additional strategy for addressing these disparities: course-based, social-psychological interventions. For example, growth-mindset interventions are designed to support students during challenging academic transitions by encouraging them to view intelligence as a flexible characteristic that can be developed through practice, rather than a fixed ability. Previous research has shown that such interventions can improve the overall performance and persistence of college students, particularly those who belong to underrepresented groups. We report a random-assignment classroom experiment, which implemented a chemistry-specific growth-mindset intervention among first-year college students enrolled in General Chemistry 1. Performance results revealed an achievement gap between underrepresented minority and white students in the control group, but no sex-based gap. Critically, after adjusting for variation in academic preparation, the mindset intervention eliminated this racial-achievement gap. Qualitative analysis of students’ written reflections from the intervention shed light on their experiences of the mindset and control treatments, deepening our understanding of mindset effects. We integrate these results with the mindset and chemical education literatures and discuss the implications for educators seeking to support underrepresented students in their own classrooms.


Physical Review Special Topics-physics Education Research | 2014

Multiyear, Multi-Instructor Evaluation of a Large-Class Interactive-Engagement Curriculum.

Michael J. Cahill; K. Mairin Hynes; Rebecca Trousil; Lisa A. Brooks; Mark A. McDaniel; Michelle D. Repice; Jiuqing Zhao; Regina F. Frey


Journals of Gerontology Series B-psychological Sciences and Social Sciences | 2011

Cognitive Exertion and Subsequent Intention Execution in Older Adults

Jill Talley Shelton; Mark A. McDaniel; Michael K. Scullin; Michael J. Cahill; Janet Singer; Gilles O. Einstein


Journal of Chemical Education | 2017

Students’ Concept-Building Approaches: A Novel Predictor of Success in Chemistry Courses

Regina F. Frey; Michael J. Cahill; Mark A. McDaniel


Physical Review Physics Education Research | 2016

Dissociative conceptual and quantitative problem solving outcomes across interactive engagement and traditional format introductory physics

Mark A. McDaniel; Siera M. Stoen; Regina F. Frey; Zachary Markow; K. Mairin Hynes; Jiuqing Zhao; Michael J. Cahill

Collaboration


Dive into the Michael J. Cahill's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mark A. McDaniel

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Regina F. Frey

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jiuqing Zhao

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

K. Mairin Hynes

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Angela Fink

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jill Talley Shelton

University of Tennessee at Chattanooga

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Julie M. Bugg

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Michelle D. Repice

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge