Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Michael S. Loop is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Michael S. Loop.


Brain Research | 1977

Visual discriminations during eyelid closure in the cat

Michael S. Loop; S. Murray Sherman

We were able to train cats raised with sutured eyelids to perform simple brightness discriminations before their lids were parted. If, and only if, a small hole was present in a lid, could some of the cats also perform a grating orientation discrimination. By establishing their thresholds for the brightness discrimination before and after dark adaptation and before and after the lids were opened, we reached three main conclusions. (1) During dark adaptation (with pupils maximally dilated and retinae most sensitive, regardless of lid suture), the cats were 3-4 log units more sensitive with the lids open than with the lids closed. This indicates a 3-4 log unit attenuation for the lids which is in agreement with our photometric measurements. (2) During light adaptation, the sensitivity difference between the conditions of opened and closed lids was only 1-2 log units. We concluded that factors (such as pupil dilatation and retinal sensitivity) partially compensated for the lid attenuation, since the open eye could have a smaller pupil and less sensitive retina during light adaptation. (3) Given these potential compensatory features of the pupil and assuming consensual pupil sizes, the deprived eye of a monocularly sutured cat may suffer more photic deprivation (since the pupil behind the closed lid would be as constricted as the pupil in the open eye) than would either eye of a binocularly sutured cat (where both pupils can be relatively large).


Psychonomic science | 1972

The effect of relative prey size on the ingestion behavior of rodent-eating snakes

Michael S. Loop; Lawrence G. Bailey

Six species of snakes from three families that represent diverse phyletic position and prey-capture techniques were fed mice of different sizes. The probability of the prey-capture technique’s being employed and of headfirst ingestion were determined by relative meal size, i.e., ingestion ratio. When dealing with relatively large prey, all constrictors and venomous species reliably located the head prior to ingestion.


Vision Research | 1994

Visual suppression and its effect upon color and luminance sensitivity

Ooi Teng Leng; Michael S. Loop

Psychophysical increment thresholds were compared for periods of phenomenological dominance or suppression produced by different stimulation of the two eyes. Three experimental procedures were used; binocular rivalry, permanent suppression and flash suppression. The amount of suppression produced by each procedure was evaluated under conditions intended to accentuate color or luminance system contribution to the detection of a spectral flash. All three procedures resulted in a different pattern of color and luminance suppression. Binocular rivalry suppressed color sensitivity more than luminance and within color, blue (439 nm) sensitivity was more suppressed than red (613 nm). Permanent suppression resulted in a similar pattern of suppression but only blue color sensitivity was reliably more suppressed than luminance sensitivity. Flash suppression produced distinctly different results such that blue color sensitivity was reliably less suppressed than luminance or red color sensitivity, which were not different from each other. Taken together these results provide clues as to where and when the physiological processes mediating visual suppression may be found in the nervous system.


Vision Research | 1982

Evidence for transient luminance and quasi-sustained color mechanisms

Steven H. Schwartz; Michael S. Loop

Reaction time distributions were determined in response to near-threshold intensity increments predicted to isolate either the opponent-color or luminance system. The reaction time histograms show a clear distinction between when the chromatic and achromatic systems detect the stimulus. Our results are consistent with previous reports suggesting the achromatic system is more sensitive to higher temporal frequencies than the chromatic system.


Vision Research | 1975

Temporal modulation sensitivity of the cat — I : Behavioral measures

Michael S. Loop; Mark A. Berkley

Abstract Critical fusion frequency (CFF) and a temporal modulation sensitivity function (TMSF) were determined for three cats using behavioral testing procedures. CFFs ranged from 40 to 55 Hz. The sensitivity functions showed the classic high frequency attenuation but had some unusual features below 15 Hz. Comparisons with the sensitivity function of humans derived with the same stimulus indicated that at the same average luminance and modulation depth, cats were capable of detecting higher rates of flicker. This difference is smaller, but not eliminated, when targets are equated for retinal illumination.


Brain Research | 1980

Visual acuity following binocular deprivation in the cat

Douglas C. Smith; Randy Lorber; R. Stanford; Michael S. Loop

Threshold visual acuity for three cats which were reared from birth to 4--12 months of age with bilateral lid closure was measured and compared to visual acuity in three cats which had the use of a non-deprived eye. The results indicate that binocular deprivation (BD) results in significant deficits in visual acuity which are proportional to the duration of deprivation. Threshold visual acuities were 3.7 cycles/deg. following 4 months of BD, 3.25 cycles/deg. following 7 months of BD and 2.55 cycles/deg. following 12 months of BD compared to acuities of 6.0, 6.5 and 6.8 cycles/deg9 for cats using a non-deprived eye. All BD cats had recovered from the initial visuomotor deficits, seen in these cats and reported in the literature, following lid-parting. The implication of such deficits in visual acuity on visual discrimination learning in BD cats is discussed.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1983

Differences in temporal appearance associated with activity in the chromatic and achromatic systems.

Steven H. Schwartz; Michael S. Loop

The temporal appearances of spatially coincident increments that activate the chromatic or achromatic system are different. Whereas near-threshold stimuli that are detected by the chromatic system appear to come on gradually, increments detected by the achromatic system appear to flash or step on. This difference in perception is consistent with previous reports that show differences in the temporal properties of the two systems.


Psychonomic science | 1969

Stimulus control of prey attack in naive rat snakes: A species duplication

Dale D. Morris; Michael S. Loop

Previously unfed, newborn rat snakes (Elaphe) were found to exhibit a greater response to prey-object extracts than to a distilled-water control stimulus. Response to the different prey-object extracts was not different. The results indicate that young rat snakes accept a wide variety of prey-objects-those normally eaten by the adult and prey of a convenien t size.


Visual Neuroscience | 2000

High color-vision sensitivity in macaque and humans

Michael S. Loop; David K. Crossman

Psychophysical (behavioral) detection thresholds and color-discrimination thresholds were determined in a macaque using a two-alternative forced-choice procedure. On a white background, detection thresholds were determined for a white increment and three spectral increments: 618, 516, and 456 nm. Intermixed with detection threshold determinations, color-discrimination thresholds were determined by presenting the white increment, and one of the spectral increments, at 1.0 log units above their respective detection thresholds and dimming both until discrimination performance fell to threshold. The monkey could discriminate the color of the increments at detection threshold because the average color-discrimination threshold was 0.98+/-0.14 log attenuation. Because the monkey was much more sensitive to the spectral increments than the white increment, we performed an unconventional experiment. We determined the monkeys detection threshold for the white increment alone, and with broadband color filters in the white light path without adjusting the lights intensity. Insertion of several color filters in the light path lowered detection thresholds of both the macaque and six human trichromats. We believe that this improvement in detection thresholds produced by simply inserting color filters in a white light path is a threshold manifestation of the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect and suggests that one of color visions important evolutionary advantages may be improved detection sensitivity.


Visual Neuroscience | 2004

Color vision sensitivity in normally dichromatic species and humans

Richard E. Van Arsdel; Michael S. Loop

Spectral-sensitivity functions for large, long-duration increments presented on a photopic white background indicate that wavelength-opponent mechanisms mediate detection in both normal and dichromatic humans. Normal humans exhibit high color-vision sensitivity as they discriminate the color of spectral flashes at detection-threshold intensities. However, dichromatic humans require stimuli up to about 0.4 log units above detection intensity to see certain colors. This low color-vision sensitivity in human dichromats may be an abnormal condition involving a defect in postreceptoral color processing. To test this hypothesis, we determined color-discrimination thresholds in normally dichromatic species: chipmunk, 13-lined ground squirrel, and tree shrew. For comparison, we also tested humans with normal and abnormal (deutan) color vision with the same apparatus and methods. Animals were trained to perform spatial two-choice discrimination tasks for food reward. Detection thresholds were determined for increments of white, 460 nm, 540 nm, 560 nm, 580 nm, 500 nm/long-pass, and 500 nm/short-pass on white backgrounds of 1.25 cd/m2, 46 cd/m2, and 130 cd/m2. Animals were also trained to respond to the colored increments when paired with the white increment when both were at equally detectable intensities. Color-discrimination thresholds were determined by dimming stimulus pairs (colored vs. white) until the subjects could no longer make the discriminations. Results indicated that the normally dichromatic species could discriminate colored stimuli from white at a mean intensity of 0.1 (+/-0.1) log units above detection threshold. The ability of normally dichromatic species to discriminate color near detection-threshold intensity is consistent with increment spectral-sensitivity functions that indicate detection by wavelength-opponent mechanisms. In keeping with previous studies, normal human trichromats discriminated color near detection-threshold intensities but humans with deutan color vision required suprathreshold intensities to discriminate the color of middle and long wavelengths. This high color-vision sensitivity of normally dichromatic species suggest that the low color-vision sensitivity in dichromatic humans is an abnormal condition and indicates a possible defect in their postreceptoral color-vision processing.

Collaboration


Dive into the Michael S. Loop's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Steven H. Schwartz

Southern College of Optometry

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Trudy J. Frey

University of Alabama at Birmingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

C. Leigh Millican

University of Alabama at Birmingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

D. J. Edwards

University of Alabama at Birmingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. B. Baldwin

University of Alabama at Birmingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Laura L. Bruce

University of Alabama at Birmingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Richard E. Van Arsdel

University of Alabama at Birmingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge