Michaela Huber
University of Colorado Boulder
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Michaela Huber.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2009
Leaf Van Boven; Katherine White; Michaela Huber
People tend to perceive immediate emotions as more intense than previous emotions. This immediacy bias in emotion perception occurred for exposure to emotional but not neutral stimuli (Study 1), when emotional stimuli were separated by both shorter (2 s; Studies 1 and 2) and longer (20 min; Studies 3, 4, and 5) delays, and for emotional reactions to pictures (Studies 1 and 2), films (Studies 3 and 4), and descriptions of terrorist threats (Study 5). The immediacy bias may be partly caused by immediate emotions salience, and by the greater availability of information about immediate compared with previous emotion. Consistent with emotional salience, when people experienced new emotions, they perceived previous emotions as less intense than they did initially (Studies 3 and 5)-a change in perception that did not occur when people did not experience a new immediate emotion (Study 2). Consistent with emotional availability, reminding people that information about emotions naturally decays from memory reduced the immediacy bias by making previous emotions seem more intense (Study 4). Discussed are implications for psychological theory and other judgments and behaviors.
PLOS ONE | 2015
Michaela Huber; Leaf Van Boven; Bernadette Park; William T. Pizzi
We examined the effects of incidental anger on perceived and actual polarization between Democrats and Republicans in the context of two national tragedies, Hurricane Katrina (Study 1) and the mass shooting that targeted Representative Gabrielle Giffords in Arizona (Study 2). We hypothesized that because of its relevance to intergroup conflict, incidental anger exacerbates the political polarization effects of issue partisanship (the correlation between partisan identification and partisan attitudes), and, separately, the correlation between conservative partisan identification and perceived polarization between Democrats and Republicans. We further hypothesized that these effects would be strongest for Republican identification because Republican leaders were targets of public criticism in both tragedies and because conservative (Republican) ideology tends to be more sensitive to threat. In the studies, participants first completed an emotion induction procedure by recalling autobiographical events that made them angry (Studies 1 & 2), sad (Studies 1 & 2), or that involved recalling emotionally neutral events (Study 2). Participants later reported their attitudes regarding the two tragedies, their perceptions of the typical Democrat’s and Republican’s attitudes on those issues, and their identification with the Democratic and Republican parties. Compared with incidental sadness (Studies 1 and 2) and a neutral condition (Study 2), incidental anger exacerbated the associations between Republican identification and partisan attitudes, and, separately between Republican identification and perceived polarization between the attitudes of Democrats and Republicans. We discuss implications for anger’s influence on political attitude formation and perceptions of group differences in political attitudes.
Psychology & Marketing | 2009
Barbara Fasolo; Ralph Hertwig; Michaela Huber; Mark Ludwig
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes | 2011
Michaela Huber; Leaf Van Boven; A. Peter McGraw; Laura Johnson-Graham
Social Justice Research | 2009
Clayton R. Critcher; Michaela Huber; Arnold K. Ho; Spassena Koleva
Archive | 2010
Michaela Huber; Leaf Van Boven; A. Peter McGraw
Journal of Social and Political Psychology | 2017
Jenny Roth; Michaela Huber; Annkatrin Juenger; James H. Liu
Archive | 2011
Charles M. Judd; Leaf Van Boven; Michaela Huber; Ana P. Nunes
Archive | 2012
Charles M. Judd; Leaf Van Boven; Michaela Huber; Ana P. Nunes
Archive | 2010
Michaela Huber