Mirja Luotonen
Oulu University Hospital
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Mirja Luotonen.
BMC Neuroscience | 2010
Eira Jansson-Verkasalo; Timo Ruusuvirta; Minna Huotilainen; Paavo Alku; Elena Kushnerenko; Kalervo Suominen; Seppo Rytky; Mirja Luotonen; Tuula Kaukola; Uolevi Tolonen; Mikko Hallman
BackgroundEarly auditory experiences are a prerequisite for speech and language acquisition. In healthy children, phoneme discrimination abilities improve for native and degrade for unfamiliar, socially irrelevant phoneme contrasts between 6 and 12 months of age as the brain tunes itself to, and specializes in the native spoken language. This process is known as perceptual narrowing, and has been found to predict normal native language acquisition. Prematurely born infants are known to be at an elevated risk for later language problems, but it remains unclear whether these problems relate to early perceptual narrowing. To address this question, we investigated early neurophysiological phoneme discrimination abilities and later language skills in prematurely born infants and in healthy, full-term infants.ResultsOur follow-up study shows for the first time that perceptual narrowing for non-native phoneme contrasts found in the healthy controls at 12 months was not observed in very prematurely born infants. An electric mismatch response of the brain indicated that whereas full-term infants gradually lost their ability to discriminate non-native phonemes from 6 to 12 months of age, prematurely born infants kept on this ability. Language performance tested at the age of 2 years showed a significant delay in the prematurely born group. Moreover, those infants who did not become specialized in native phonemes at the age of one year, performed worse in the communicative language test (MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories) at the age of two years. Thus, decline in sensitivity to non-native phonemes served as a predictor for further language development.ConclusionOur data suggest that detrimental effects of prematurity on language skills are based on the low degree of specialization to native language early in development. Moreover, delayed or atypical perceptual narrowing was associated with slower language acquisition. The results hence suggest that language problems related to prematurity may partially originate already from this early tuning stage of language acquisition.
Laryngoscope | 2010
Tuija Löppönen Md; Marja-Leena Väisänen; Mirja Luotonen; Minna Allinen; Johanna Uusimaa; Päivi Lindholm; Elina Mäki-Torkko; Mirja Väyrynen; Heikki Löppönen; Jaakko Leisti
Objective: The aims of the present study were to evaluate the role of the gap junction protein β‐2 gene (GJB2), encoding connexin 26 (Cx26), in children with moderate to profound prelingual nonsyndromic sensorineural hearing impairment (HI) and to investigate the carrier frequencies of the GJB2 gene mutations in a control population in Northern Finland. Methods: Mutation analysis was performed by direct sequencing and carrier detection by conformation sensitive gel electrophoresis further confirmed by direct sequencing. Results: Cx26 mutations were found in 15 of 71 (21.1%) (67 families) children with HI. Homozygosity for the mutation 35delG was shown to be the cause of HI in 13 of 15 (86.7%) children. Homozygosity for the M34T genotype was found in one child, and compound heterozygosity for the M34T/V37I genotype was found in another. Five families of those with suspected familial HI (29.4%) and six families out of those with sporadic HI (12.0%) had a homozygous or compound heterozygous mutation. The carrier frequency for the mutation 35delG was 1 of 78 (4 of 313) and that for the M34T was 1 of 26 (12 of 313). Conclusion: 35delG/35delG genotype was found to be a significant cause of moderate to profound prelingual nonsyndromic sensorineural HI in Northern Finland. M34T/M34T genotype was seen in only one child, but the carrier frequency of the M34T allele was about three times higher than that of the 35delG mutation.
British Journal of Audiology | 1984
Martti Sorri; Mirja Luotonen; Kyösti Laitakari
A hundred and fifty hearing-handicapped were interviewed at home two years after their hearing aids had been fitted. The aim was to find out how many of the delivered aids were in use, and if not, why. A remarkable percentage of the hearing aids were very seldom, if ever, in use (23%), 57% of them were used regularly every day and 19% of the hearing-handicapped used their aids occasionally. The hearing aids were less used if the hearing loss was mild or sensorineural. Behind-the-ear aids were used more regularly than the body-worn aids. The time needed for learning to handle the aid did not affect the amount of use. Very often, despite being satisfied with the instruction, the non-users were not skilled in handling the aid. They also often complained about having little opportunity to converse with others; however, no difference in the amount of social contacts could be shown between the users and non-users. On the basis of the results of this study the policy of our hearing-aid fitting has been revised.
Journal of Child Language | 2011
Sari Kunnari; Tuula Savinainen-Makkonen; Laurence B. Leonard; Leena Mäkinen; Anna-Kaisa Tolonen; Mirja Luotonen; Eeva K Leinonen
Children with specific language impairment (SLI) vary widely in their ability to use tense/agreement inflections depending on the type of language being acquired, a fact that current accounts of SLI have tried to explain. Finnish provides an important test case for these accounts because: (1) verbs in the first and second person permit null subjects whereas verbs in the third person do not; and (2) tense and agreement inflections are agglutinating and thus one type of inflection can appear without the other. Probes were used to compare the verb inflection use of Finnish-speaking children with SLI, and both age-matched and younger typically developing children. The children with SLI were less accurate, and the pattern of their errors did not match predictions based on current accounts of SLI. It appears that children with SLI have difficulty learning complex verb inflection paradigms apart from any problem specific to tense and agreement.
Audiology and Neuro-otology | 2013
Sanna Häkli; Mirja Luotonen; Martti Sorri; Kari Majamaa
The age at onset and the severity of hearing impairment (HI) varies widely among subjects and within families with the m.1555A>G mutation in mitochondrial DNA. We examined prospectively the hearing of 19 children in three nuclear families of a pedigree with m.1555A>G during a period of 7.8 years. The children underwent an audiological examination annually. At the end of the follow-up, the children were 2–13 years old. The parents were asked about the exposure of the children to risk factors of HI. We found that the 19 children with m.1555A>G were born with normal hearing and that 10 of them had developed HI by the end of the follow-up. High frequencies were affected first. The median age at the onset of HI was 3.7 years. Both the severity of HI and the age of onset varied within and between families. Most commonly, audiograms revealed a sensorineural, progressive HI sloping towards high frequencies. We could not identify environmental factors which could modify the development of HI. In conclusion, we were able to pinpoint the time of onset of HI and to follow the progression of HI in childhood. Our results show that there are distinct phenotypes, but at present there are no means to predict which phenotype will develop. It is important to follow up the hearing of children in families with the m.1555A>G mutation, because these children generally pass the newborn hearing screening, and the age at onset or the phenotype of HI cannot be predicted.
Applied Psycholinguistics | 2014
Laurence B. Leonard; Sari Kunnari; Tuula Savinainen-Makkonen; Anna-Kaisa Tolonen; Leena Mäkinen; Mirja Luotonen; Eeva K Leinonen
Finnish-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI, N = 15, M age = 5;2), a group of same-age typically developing peers (TD-A, N = 15, M age = 5;2) and a group of younger typically developing children (TD-Y, N = 15, M age = 3;8) were compared in their use of accusative, partitive, and genitive case noun suffixes. The children with SLI were less accurate than both groups of TD children in case marking, suggesting that their difficulties with agreement extend to grammatical case. However, these children were also less accurate in making the phonological changes in the stem needed for suffixation. This second type of error suggests that problems in morphophonology may constitute a separate problem in Finnish SLI.
International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology | 2014
Sanna Häkli; Mirja Luotonen; Risto Bloigu; Kari Majamaa; Martti Sorri
OBJECTIVES The purpose of this study was to determine the prevalence and etiology of hearing impairment (HI) in Finnish children and to evaluate the frequency and type of additional disabilities among children with HI. METHODS Subjects consisted of 214 children with mild to profound HI ascertained until the age of 10 years. They belonged to the birth cohort spanning the years 1993-2002 in northern Finland. The clinical data were collected from the electronic patient records of the Oulu University Hospital. Age at ascertainment, degree and type of HI and audiogram configuration were determined. Risk factors and etiology of HI and co-existing disabilities were recorded. RESULTS The prevalence of childhood HI was 2.3/1000 live births (95% CI; 2.0, 2.7). The etiology of HI was genetic in 47.2%, acquired in 16.4% and unknown in 36.4% children. Among the 214 children with HI, 101 (47.2%) had other minor or major disabilities. The frequency of additional disabilities did not differ between children with mild HI and those with moderate or severe HI (p=0.78). Additional disabilities were more common (65.7%) in children with acquired HI than in children with genetic or unknown HI (43.6%) (p=0.035). CONCLUSION The prevalence of childhood HI has remained unchanged in northern Finland as compared to previous studies. Genetic causes were the most common (47%) etiology of childhood HI. Among acquired causes of HI, perinatal risk factors were more common than previously. The frequency of additional disabilities was similar among children with different degrees of HI. Because almost 40% of children had one or more additional disabilities affecting development or learning, it is important to take them into consideration in rehabilitation.
International Journal of Psychophysiology | 2015
Leena Ervast; Jarmo A. Hämäläinen; Swantje Zachau; Kaisa Lohvansuu; Kaisu Heinänen; Mari Veijola; Elisa Heikkinen; Kalervo Suominen; Mirja Luotonen; Matti Lehtihalmes; Paavo H. T. Leppänen
The brains ability to recognize different acoustic cues (e.g., frequency changes in rapid temporal succession) is important for speech perception and thus for successful language development. Here we report on distinct event-related potentials (ERPs) in 5-6-year-old children recorded in a passive oddball paradigm to repeated tone pair stimuli with a frequency change in the second tone in the pair, replicating earlier findings. An occasional insertion of a third tone within the tone pair generated a more merged pattern, which has not been reported previously in 5-6-year-old children. Both types of deviations elicited pre-attentive discriminative mismatch negativity (MMN) and late discriminative negativity (LDN) responses. Temporal principal component analysis (tPCA) showed a similar topographical pattern with fronto-central negativity for MMN and LDN. We also found a previously unreported discriminative response complex (P340-N440) at the temporal electrode sites at about 140 ms and 240 ms after the frequency deviance, which we suggest reflects a discriminative processing of frequency change. The P340 response was positive with a clear radial distribution preceding the fronto-central frequency MMN by about 30 ms. The results indicate that 5-6-year-old children can detect frequency change and the occasional insertion of an additional tone in sound pairs as reflected by MMN and LDN, even with quite short within-stimulus intervals (150 ms and 50 ms). Furthermore, MMN for these changes is preceded by another response to deviancy, temporal P340, which seems to reflect a parallel but earlier discriminatory process.
BMC Medical Genetics | 2015
Sanna Häkli; Mirja Luotonen; Martti Sorri; Kari Majamaa
BackgroundMutations in the two MT-RNR genes in mitochondrial DNA can cause hearing impairment that presents with variable severity and age of onset. In order to study the prevalence of mutations in MT-RNR1 and MT-RNR2 genes among Finnish children, we studied a ten-year cohort of hearing impaired children born in Northern Finland.MethodsWe studied children, who had been born in Northern Finland in 1993–2002 and who had been ascertained to have hearing impairment by 31 December 2007. Samples from 103 children were sequenced in order to find mutations in the MT-RNR1 and MT-RNR2 genes.ResultsOne child harboured the pathogenic m.1555A > G mutation in MT-RNR1 suggesting a frequency of 4.4/100,000 in the Finnish paediatric population. In addition, eight rare variants and 13 polymorphisms were found in MT-RNR1 and MT-RNR2 genes. Five of the rare variants were deemed to be haplogroup-specific polymorphisms rather than putative pathogenic mutations, while the remaining three variants have been reported in various haplogroups. Among them m.990 T > C occurs at a conserved site.ConclusionsThe presence of m.990 T > C variant in various haplogroups and the rather high degree of conservation at this site suggest that this transition is a pathogenic rather than homoplasic neutral variant. Identification of further patients with m.990 T > C and segregation analysis in their families should help in determining the pathogenic potential of this variant.
Folia Phoniatrica Et Logopaedica | 1998
Mirja Luotonen; Matti Uhari; Marja Uhari; Lempi Aitola; Aino-Maija Lukkaroinen; Jukka Luotonen
The aim of the study was to evaluate the possible differences in linguistic skills and school achievement between girls and boys. In a nation-wide study, the achievement of 1,936 second-grade Finnish girls and boys was appraised according to the teachers’ rating. The results were compared with the linguistic test performance of 364 pupils from the nearby area. The mean rates of girls in writing, reading, oral performance and attention were significantly higher than those of boys as evaluated by the teachers (p < 0.01). Also, the risk of boys to be below the median in their achievement rates compared with girls was higher (risk ratios 1.3–1.6, 95% confidence intervals 1.2–1.7, p < 0.01). In multiple regression analysis where birth order, duration of nursery day care, number of otitis media episodes and parental education were controlled for, male gender proved to be a risk factor for linguistic skills as graded by the teachers. Yet, performance in linguistic tests did not support the superiority of girls in linguistic skills. Professionals taking care of linguistic problems in children should be aware that teachers’ evaluations are influenced by the gender of the pupil.