Nancy A. Smider
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Nancy A. Smider.
Child Development | 2002
Kathryn S. Lemery; Marilyn J. Essex; Nancy A. Smider
This study examined the hypothesis that item overlap, or measurement confounding, accounts for the correlation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms in children. First, a conceptual approach was taken in which 41 experts rated temperament (Childrens Behavior Questionnaire, CBQ) and behavior problem symptom items (Preschool Behavior Questionnaire, PBQ) for their fit to both constructs. With this approach, 10% of temperament and 38% of symptom items were confounded. Second, an empirical approach was taken and CBQ and PBQ items were factor analyzed with data from a multi-informant longitudinal study of 451 children. Using this method, 9% of temperament and 23% of symptom items were confounded. Most importantly, removing the confounded items from the CBQ and PBQ scales did not affect the relation between temperament and symptoms, suggesting that the associations were not due to measurement confounding. In addition, the predictive power of earlier temperament for DSM-IV symptoms (Health and Behavior Questionnaire) remained high with the purified CBQ scale. The findings of this study contribute to the understanding of the relation between normal-range temperament and extreme behavior.
Psychological Medicine | 1998
M. G. Gittleman; Marjorie H. Klein; Nancy A. Smider; Marilyn J. Essex
BACKGROUND Attachment theory posits links between early experiences with parents, adult relationships and adult mental health, but does not specify whether these are independent, mediating, or moderating effects. METHODS Associations of parents behaviour on the Parental Bonding Instrument, adult attachment styles and three dimensions of mental health were investigated in a large sample of women and men. RESULTS Men and women with secure styles recalled higher levels of care from both parents than those with fearful styles. Maternal and paternal control were more consistent predictors of increased distress for men than for women. Fearful and preoccupied adult styles were associated with higher levels of distress in both men and women. While adult styles had few mediating effects on the association of parental behaviour and mental health, interactions between the fearful style and parental variables suggested that this form of insecurity sometimes accentuated the impact of high parental care or low paternal control on mental health in both men and women; among women, however, the secure style seemed to buffer somewhat the negative effect of high parental control. CONCLUSION Although the amount of variance explained by either parental behaviour or adult styles was modest, patterns of moderating effects of adult styles on associations between parental behaviour and mental health suggested that both continuity and discontinuity principles can be applied to understanding these links.
Developmental Psychology | 1999
Nazan Aksan; H. Hill Goldsmith; Nancy A. Smider; Marilyn J. Essex; Roseanne Clark; Janet Shibley Hyde; Marjorie H. Klein; Deborah Lowe Vandell
The number and nature of temperamental types in 488 children aged 3 years 6 months was examined on the basis of a broad set of temperamental characteristics, including positive and negative emotionality and the attentional and behavioral control domains. Configural frequency analysis methods showed clear support for two temperament types: controlled-nonexpressive and noncontrolled-expressive. These types showed meaningful differences against external criteria related to a wide range of problem behaviors from the emotional, social, and attentional domains. The reports of problem behaviors were obtained contemporaneously from fathers and caregivers. These findings replicated a year later when children were aged 4 years 6 months. Furthermore, the findings showed that infant and toddler-age temperamental characteristics differentiated these preschool-aged types. The authors discuss the implications of the results for a categorical view of temperament-personality.
Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers | 2003
Marilyn J. Essex; H. Hill Goldsmith; Nancy A. Smider; Isa Dolski; Steven K. Sutton; Richard J. Davidson
We investigated the reliability and validity of a video-based method of measuring the magnitude of children’s emotion-modulated startle response when electromyographic (EMG) measurement is not feasible. Thirty-one children between the ages of 4 and 7 years were videotaped while watching short video clips designed to elicit happiness or fear. Embedded in the audio track of the video clips were acoustic startle probes. A coding system was developed to quantify from the video record the strength of the eye-blink startle response to the probes. EMG measurement of the eye blink was obtained simultaneously. Intercoder reliability for the video coding was high (Cohen’sκ = .90). The average within-subjects probe-by-probe correlation between the EMG- and video-based methods was .84. Group-level correlations between the methods were also strong, and there was some evidence of emotion modulation of the startle response with both the EMG- and the video-derived data. Although the video method cannot be used to assess the latency, probability, or duration of startle blinks, the findings indicate that it can serve as a valid proxy of EMG in the assessment of the magnitude of emotion-modulated startle in studies of children conducted outside of a laboratory setting, where traditional psychophysiological methods are not feasible.
British Journal of Psychiatry | 2001
W. Thomas Boyce; Jodi A. Quas; Abbey Alkon; Nancy A. Smider; Marilyn J. Essex; David J. Kupfer
Child Development | 2002
Nancy A. Smider; Marilyn J. Essex; Ned H. Kalin; Kristin A. Buss; Marjorie H. Klein; Richard J. Davidson; H. Hill Goldsmith
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 1999
Jennifer C. Ablow; Jeffrey R. Measelle; Helena C. Kraemer; Richard Harrington; Joan L. Luby; Nancy A. Smider; Lisa Dierker; Valerie Clark; Bernadka W. Dubicka; Amy Heffelfinger; Marilyn J. Essex; David J. Kupfer
British Journal of Psychiatry | 2001
Marilyn J. Essex; Marjorie H. Klein; Richard A. Miech; Nancy A. Smider
Developmental Psychobiology | 2003
Abbey Alkon; Lauren Heim Goldstein; Nancy A. Smider; Marilyn J. Essex; David J. Kupfer; W. Thomas Boyce
Psychology and Aging | 1996
Nancy A. Smider; Marilyn J. Essex; Carol D. Ryff