Nele Wild-Wall
Technical University of Dortmund
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Nele Wild-Wall.
Brain Research | 2008
Nele Wild-Wall; Michael Falkenstein; Joachim Hohnsbein
The present study investigates behavioural and event-related potential (ERP) differences between young and older participants in two variants of a flanker task. Flankers preceded the target by 100 ms (Experiment 1) or were presented simultaneously with the target (Experiment 2). In both experiments the response times showed an age-related slowing and a compatibility effect, which did not differ significantly across age. The older participants committed only half as many errors as the young ones. The visual ERPs revealed that the speed of visual perception was similar between groups. In addition the processing of the targets, but not of the flankers, appeared to be enhanced in the older participants. Moreover the lateralized readiness potential (LRP) started later and was larger for old vs. young participants, which was most pronounced for the incorrect LRP activation due to the flankers. The LRP amplitude effect is due to an enhanced activation over contralateral motor areas, which appears to be a general finding unrelated to the error rate. In summary, in the present study we could not find evidence for enhanced flanker interference in the performance of older compared to young participants. The reduced error rates for older participants are likely due to enhanced processing of the targets and delayed transmission of flanker information from visual to motor areas.
BMC Psychiatry | 2006
Robert D. Oades; Nele Wild-Wall; Stephanie A. Juran; Jan Sachsse; Ljubov B. Oknina; Bernd Röpcke
BackgroundThe event-related brain response mismatch negativity (MMN) registers changes in auditory stimulation with temporal lobe sources reflecting short-term echoic memory and frontal sources a deviance-induced switch in processing. Impairment, controversially present at the onset of schizophrenia, develops rapidly and can remain independent of clinical improvement. We examined the characteristics of the scalp-recorded MMN and related these to tests of short-term memory and set-shifting. We assessed whether the equivalent dipole sources are affected already at illness-onset in adolescence and how these features differ after a 14-year course following an adolescent onset. The strength, latency, orientation and location of frontal and temporal lobe sources of MMN activity early and late in the course of adolescent-onset schizophrenia are analysed and illustrated.MethodsMMN, a measure of auditory change-detection, was elicited by short deviant tones in a 3-tone oddball-presentation and recorded from 32 scalp electrodes. Four dipole sources were placed following hypothesis-led calculations using brain electrical source analysis on brain atlas and MR-images. A short neuropsychological test battery was administered. We compared 28 adolescent patients with a first episode of schizophrenia and 18 patients 14 years after diagnosis in adolescence with two age-matched control groups from the community (n = 22 and 18, respectively).ResultsMMN peaked earlier in the younger than the older subjects. The amplitude was reduced in patients, especially the younger group, and was here associated with negative symptoms and slow set-shifting. In first-episode patients the temporal lobe sources were more ventral than in controls, while the left cingular and right inferior-mid frontal sources were more caudal. In the older patients the left temporal locus remained ventral (developmental stasis), the right temporal locus extended more antero-laterally (illness progression), and the right frontal source moved antero-laterally (normalised).ConclusionFrom the start of the illness there were differences in the dipole-model between healthy and patient groups. Separate characteristics of the sources of the activity differences showed an improvement, stasis or deterioration with illness-duration. The precise nature of the changes in the sources of MMN activity and their relationship to selective information processing and storage depend on the specific psychopathology and heterogeneous course of the illness.
Biological Psychology | 2010
Patrick D. Gajewski; Nele Wild-Wall; Sergei A. Schapkin; Udo Erdmann; Gabriele Freude; Michael Falkenstein
In a cross-sectional, electrophysiological study 91 workers of a big car factory performed a series of switch tasks to assess their cognitive control functions. Four groups of workers participated in the study: 23 young and 23 middle aged assembly line employees and 22 young and 23 middle aged employees with flexible job demands like service and maintenance. Participants performed three digit categorisation tasks. In addition to single task blocks, a cue-based (externally guided) and a memory-based (internally guided) task switch block was administered. Compared to young participants, older ones showed the typical RT-decline. No differences between younger and older participants regarding the local switch costs could be detected despite the source of the current task information. In contrast, whereas the groups did not differ in mixing costs in the cued condition, clear performance decrements in the memory-based mixing block were observed in the group of older employees with repetitive work demands. These findings were corroborated by a number of electrophysiological results showing a reduced CNV suggesting an impairment of task specific preparation, an attenuated P3b suggesting reduced working memory capacity and a decreased Ne suggesting deficits in error monitoring in older participants with repetitive job demands. The results are compatible with the assumption that long lasting, unchallenging job demands may induce several neurocognitive impairments which are already evident in the early fifties. Longitudinal studies are needed to confirm this assumption.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2007
Nele Wild-Wall; Joachim Hohnsbein; Michael Falkenstein
OBJECTIVE The anticipation of complex cognitive tasks involves effortful preparation being reflected in the contingent negative variation (CNV) of the event-related potential. In the literature there are contradictory results concerning the effect of age on this potential. We wanted to investigate effects of age, time-on-task, and task difficulty on the CNV. METHOD Young and middle-aged participants performed a visual search and a non-search task during an early and a late phase of a 6-h session. RESULTS Performance data revealed increased response times and error rates for middle-aged vs. young participants. Most importantly, an increased frontal CNV amplitude was found for the older participants, especially pronounced in the search task. A late positivity which was elicited to the offset of the preceding stimulus was increased for the middle-aged vs. young group in the visual search task only. There was no effect of time-on-task on performance, but the CNV became larger with time-on-task in the search task while it became smaller in the non-search task. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest an enhancement of effortful task preparation for middle-aged participants especially when the task is difficult. SIGNIFICANCE This underlines the role of the CNV as a neurophysiological indicator for effortful cognitive preparation.
Schizophrenia Research | 2005
Ljubov B. Oknina; Nele Wild-Wall; Robert D. Oades; Stephanie A. Juran; Bernd Röpcke; Ute Pfueller; Matthias Weisbrod; Esther W. Chan; Eric Y.H. Chen
Mismatch negativity (MMN) is an event-related potential measure of auditory change detection. It is widely reported to be smaller in patients with schizophrenia and may not improve along with otherwise successful clinical treatment. The main aim of this report is to explore ways of measuring and presenting four features of frequency-deviant MMN dipole sources (dipole moment, peak latency, brain location and orientation) and to relate these to the processes of psychopathology and illness progression. Data from early onset patients (EOS) at the start of the illness in adolescence, and others who had their first break in adolescence 15 years ago (S-15Y) were compared with two groups of age-matched healthy controls (C-EOS, C-15Y). A four-source model fitted the MMN waveform recorded from all four groups, whether MMN amplitude was more (EOS) or less (S-15Y) reduced. The locations were in the left superior temporal and anterior cingulate gyri, right superior temporal and inferior/mid frontal cortices. Dipole latencies confirmed a bottom-up sequence of processing and dipole moments were larger in the temporal lobes and on the left. Patients showed small dipole location changes that were more marked in the S-15Y than the EOS group (more rostral for the left anterior cingulate, more caudal for the right mid-frontal dipole) consistent with illness progression. The modelling of MMN dipole sources on brain atlas and anatomical images suggests that there is a degree of dissociation during illness between small progressive anatomical changes and some functional recovery indexed by scalp recordings from patients with an onset in adolescence 15 years before compared to adolescents in their first episode.
International Journal of Psychophysiology | 2009
Nele Wild-Wall; Robert D. Oades; Marion Schmidt-Wessels; Hanna Christiansen; Michael Falkenstein
This study used event-related potentials (ERPs) and flanker-task performance to compare executive functions in adolescents with ADHD, their siblings and independent healthy controls. The aim was to investigate the processing of distracting stimuli, control over inappropriate responses, and the detection of errors in the presence of incompatible and No-go stimuli (arrow-heads and a circle, respectively). Performance showed no major differences between the groups, although No-go errors were numerically increased for the patients. Adolescents with ADHD were not characterised by the absence of post-error response slowing. The ADHD group showed a generally smaller N2 and a missing amplification of the frontal P3 (P3a) in No-go vs. incompatible trials most likely reflecting impaired inhibitory processing. In response-locked potentials error-related negativity (Ne) and positivity (Pe) did not clearly differentiate between the groups. This study shows that ADHD children are more impaired in controlled than automatic response processing and inhibition. This was particularly evident in reduced frontal activity in general and especially after No-go stimuli. Deficient error processing may, however, not be a cardinal feature of adolescents with ADHD. Future work must orient towards determining if there is a subgroup for whom the inhibitory impairment is characteristic.
Biological Psychology | 2008
Nele Wild-Wall; Olaf Dimigen; Werner Sommer
There is mounting evidence that under some conditions the processing of facial identity and facial emotional expressions may not be independent; however, the nature of this interaction remains to be established. By using event-related brain potentials (ERP) we attempted to localize these interactions within the information processing system. During an expression discrimination task (Experiment 1) categorization was faster for portraits of personally familiar vs. unfamiliar persons displaying happiness. The peak latency of the P300 (trend) and the onset of the stimulus-locked LRP were shorter for familiar than unfamiliar faces. This implies a late perceptual but pre-motoric locus of the facilitating effect of familiarity on expression categorization. In Experiment 2 participants performed familiarity decisions about portraits expressing different emotions. Results revealed an advantage of happiness over disgust specifically for familiar faces. The facilitation was localized in the response selection stage as suggested by a shorter onset of the LRP. Both experiments indicate that familiarity and facial expression may not be independent processes. However, depending on the kind of decision different processing stages may be facilitated for happy familiar faces.
Psychophysiology | 2003
Nele Wild-Wall; Jörg Sangals; Werner Sommer; Hartmut Leuthold
Ulrich, Leuthold, and Sommer (1998) suggested that movement preparation at the level of the motor cortex, as indexed by the lateralized readiness potential (LRP), proceeds in a strongly hierarchical fashion, where parameters other than response hand are prepared only if all the movement parameters are known. These conclusions were based on an experiment where a precue provided information about response hand, direction of finger movement, and movement force. To assess the generality of these findings, we replaced the force parameter with response finger. LRP indicated that preparation of the required finger is possible even when preliminary information is incomplete. Therefore, movement preparation appears to follow different rules when anatomical relationships (hand and finger) are concerned as compared to functional parameters like movement direction. On the other hand, at a limb-unspecific level, as indicated by the contingent negative variation, we confirmed evidence for parallel programming of all movement parameters.
Neuroscience Letters | 2008
Nele Wild-Wall; Rita Willemssen; Michael Falkenstein; Christian Beste
A decline in motor performance and timing performance is evident not only in clinical patient groups, e.g. with Parkinsons or Huntingtons disease but also in normal ageing. Common to the mentioned groups is a deterioration in dopaminergic function of fronto-striatal brain circuits. These areas belong to a distributed network in the brain playing an important role in time perception and timing behaviour. Therefore, we measured time estimation performance in five groups of healthy young and healthy old participants, of patients with Parkinsons disease (PD), with presymptomatic and symptomatic Huntingtons disease (HD). Participants were instructed to indicate by a precise button press when 1.2s had elapsed after stimulus onset. They received feedback after correct (within a specified time window) or incorrect responses. When compared to the young control group the performance in old participants, patients with Parkinsons, presymptomatic and symptomatic Huntingtons disease was inferior, while differences were not noticed between the latter four groups. The data underline the importance of fronto-striatal circuits in the brain for time processing and time estimation. It is suggested that it is not the degree of dysfunction of the fronto-striatal dopamine system but rather the mere existence of a dysfunction, even if subtle, which is pivotal for a decline in timing performance. A time estimation task can serve as a useful tool to detect even faint changes in the integrity of the fronto-striatal dopamine system.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2009
Nele Wild-Wall; Rita Willemssen; Michael Falkenstein
OBJECTIVE We examined whether the age-related decline in the integrity of the mid-brain dopamine system plays a role in the adaptation of precisely timed motor responses by feedback information. METHODS Participants performed a time-production task with feedback given after each trial. They were encouraged to use the feedback for improving temporal response accuracy. Event-related potentials to the feedback were analyzed. RESULTS Older participants performed poorer than the young. Overall, high response accuracy was more favoured following positive than negative feedback. Older participants performed even worse after negative feedback than the younger. The feedback-related negativity (FRN) was of lower amplitude for older vs. young participants. FRN amplitude correlated significantly with response accuracy only for the young group. CONCLUSIONS The decreased response accuracy during time production of the older group may be related to a weakened fronto-striatal dopamine system and thus a reduced ability to use feedback information for improving temporal aspects of the motor response. SIGNIFICANCE The study points to difficulties especially for older participants to process error feedback, and it underlines the importance of positive feedback in order to improve temporal response accuracy. Suggestions can be drawn for the design of feedback information especially for older adults in motor learning environments.