Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel.


Vision Research | 1999

Does early non-linearity account for second-order motion?

Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel; Mark A. Georgeson

A contrast-modulated (CM) pattern is formed when a modulating or envelope function imposes local contrast variations on a higher-frequency carrier. Motion may be seen when the envelope drifts across a stationary carrier and this has been attributed to a second-order pathway for motion. However, an early compressive response to luminance (e.g. in the photoreceptors) would introduce a distortion product at the modulating frequency. We used a nulling method to measure the distortion product, and then asked whether this early distortion could account for perception of second-order motion. The first stimulus sequence consisted of alternate frames of CM (100% modulation) and luminance-modulated (LM) patterns. Carriers were either 2-D binary noise (4 x 4 min arc dots) or a 4 c/deg grating, both modulated at 0.6 c/deg. The carrier was stationary while the phase of the modulating signal (LM alternating with CM) stepped successively through 90 degrees to the left or right. Motion was seen in a direction opposite to the phase stepping, consistent with early compressive distortion that induces an out-of-phase LM component into the CM stimulus. We measured distortion amplitude by adding LM to the CM frames to null the perceived motion. Distortion increased as the square of carrier contrast, as predicted by the compressive transducer. It also increased with modulation drift rate, implying that the transducer is time-dependent, not static. Thus early compressive non-linearity does induce first-order artefacts into second-order stimuli. Nevertheless this does not account for second-order motion, since perceived motion of second-order sequences (CM in every frame) could in general not be nulled by adding LM components. We conclude that two pathways for motion do exist.


Vision Research | 2005

Idiosyncratic initiation of saccadic face exploration in humans

Ute Leonards; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

Visual processing and subsequent action are limited by the effectiveness of eye movement control: where the eyes fixate determines what part of the visual environment is seen in detail. Visual exploration consists of stereotypical sequences of saccadic eye movements which are known to depend upon both external factors, such as visual stimulus features, and internal cognition-related factors, such as attention and memory. However, how these two factors are balanced is unknown. One determinant might be the familiarity or ecological importance of the visual stimulus being explored. Recordings of saccades for human face stimuli revealed that their exploration was subject to strong individual biases for the initial saccade direction: subjects tended to look first to one particular side. We attribute this to internal factors. In contrast, exploration of landscapes, fractals or inverted faces showed no significant direction bias for initial saccades, suggesting more externally driven exploration patterns. Thus the balance between external and internal factors in scene exploration depends on stimulus type. An analysis of saccade latencies suggested that this individual preference for first saccade direction during face exploration leads to higher effectiveness through automation. The findings have implications for the understanding of both normal and abnormal eye movements.


PLOS ONE | 2011

Dazzle Camouflage Affects Speed Perception

Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel; Roland Baddeley; Chloe E. Palmer; Innes C. Cuthill

Movement is the enemy of camouflage: most attempts at concealment are disrupted by motion of the target. Faced with this problem, navies in both World Wars in the twentieth century painted their warships with high contrast geometric patterns: so-called “dazzle camouflage”. Rather than attempting to hide individual units, it was claimed that this patterning would disrupt the perception of their range, heading, size, shape and speed, and hence reduce losses from, in particular, torpedo attacks by submarines. Similar arguments had been advanced earlier for biological camouflage. Whilst there are good reasons to believe that most of these perceptual distortions may have occurred, there is no evidence for the last claim: changing perceived speed. Here we show that dazzle patterns can distort speed perception, and that this effect is greatest at high speeds. The effect should obtain in predators launching ballistic attacks against rapidly moving prey, or modern, low-tech battlefields where handheld weapons are fired from short ranges against moving vehicles. In the latter case, we demonstrate that in a typical situation involving an RPG7 attack on a Land Rover the reduction in perceived speed is sufficient to make the grenade miss where it was aimed by about a metre, which could be the difference between survival or not for the occupants of the vehicle.


Vision Research | 1999

Motion contrast: a new metric for direction discrimination

Mark A. Georgeson; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

The Adelson-Bergen energy model (Adelson, E. H., & Bergen, J. R. (1985). Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 2, 284-299) is a standard framework for understanding first-order motion processing. The opponent energy for a given input is calculated by subtracting one directional energy measure (EL) from its opposite (ER), and its sign indicates the direction of motion of the input. Our observers viewed a dynamic sequence of gratings (1 c/deg) equivalent to the sum of two gratings moving in opposite directions with different contrasts. The ratio of contrasts was varied across trials. We found that opponent energy was a very poor predictor of direction discrimination performance. Heeger (1992). Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex. Visual Neuroscience, 9, 181-197) has suggested that divisive inhibition amongst striate cells requires a contrast gain control in the energy model. A new metric can be formulated in the spirit of Heegers model by normalising the opponent energy (EL - ER) with flicker energy, the sum of the directional motion energies (EL + ER). This new measure, motion contrast (EL - ER)/(EL + ER), was found to be a good predictor of direction discrimination performance over a wide range of contrast levels, but opponent energy was not. Discrimination thresholds expressed as motion contrast were around 0.5 +/- 0.1 for the sampled drifting gratings used in our experiments. We show that the dependence on motion contrast, and the threshold of about 0.5, can be predicted by a modified opponent energy model based on current knowledge of the response functions and response variance of cortical cells.


PLOS ONE | 2012

Glass Shape Influences Consumption Rate for Alcoholic Beverages

Angela S. Attwood; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel; George Stothart; Marcus R. Munafò

Background High levels of alcohol consumption and increases in heavy episodic drinking (binge drinking) are a growing public concern, due to their association with increased risk of personal and societal harm. Alcohol consumption has been shown to be sensitive to factors such as price and availability. The aim of this study was to explore the influence of glass shape on the rate of consumption of alcoholic and non-alcoholic beverages. Methods This was an experimental design with beverage (lager, soft drink), glass (straight, curved) and quantity (6 fl oz, 12 fl oz) as between-subjects factors. Social male and female alcohol consumers (n = 159) attended two experimental sessions, and were randomised to drink either lager or a soft drink from either a curved or straight-sided glass, and complete a computerised task identifying perceived midpoint of the two glasses (order counterbalanced). Ethical approval was granted by the Faculty of Science Research Ethics Committee at the University of Bristol. The primary outcome measures were total drinking time of an alcoholic or non-alcoholic beverage, and perceptual judgement of the half-way point of a straight and curved glass. Results Participants were 60% slower to consume an alcoholic beverage from a straight glass compared to a curved glass. This effect was only observed for a full glass and not a half-full glass, and was not observed for a non-alcoholic beverage. Participants also misjudged the half-way point of a curved glass to a greater degree than that of a straight glass, and there was a trend towards a positive association between the degree of error and total drinking time. Conclusions Glass shape appears to influence the rate of drinking of alcoholic beverages. This may represent a modifiable target for public health interventions.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2013

Camouflage, detection and identification of moving targets

Joanna Hall; Innes C. Cuthill; Roland Baddeley; Adam Shohet; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

Nearly all research on camouflage has investigated its effectiveness for concealing stationary objects. However, animals have to move, and patterns that only work when the subject is static will heavily constrain behaviour. We investigated the effects of different camouflages on the three stages of predation—detection, identification and capture—in a computer-based task with humans. An initial experiment tested seven camouflage strategies on static stimuli. In line with previous literature, background-matching and disruptive patterns were found to be most successful. Experiment 2 showed that if stimuli move, an isolated moving object on a stationary background cannot avoid detection or capture regardless of the type of camouflage. Experiment 3 used an identification task and showed that while camouflage is unable to slow detection or capture, camouflaged targets are harder to identify than uncamouflaged targets when similar background objects are present. The specific details of the camouflage patterns have little impact on this effect. If one has to move, camouflage cannot impede detection; but if one is surrounded by similar targets (e.g. other animals in a herd, or moving background distractors), then camouflage can slow identification. Despite previous assumptions, motion does not entirely ‘break’ camouflage.


The American Naturalist | 2012

A quantitative test of the predicted relationship between countershading and lighting environment.

William L. Allen; Roland Baddeley; Innes C. Cuthill; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

Countershading, a vertical luminance gradient from a dark back to a light belly, is perhaps the most common coloration phenotype in the animal kingdom. Why? We investigated whether countershading functions as self-shadow concealment (SSC) in ruminants. We calculated “optimal” countershading for SSC by measuring illumination falling onto a model ruminant as a function of time of day and lighting environment. Calibrated images of 114 species of ruminant were compared to the countershading model, and phylogenetic analyses were used to find the best predictors of coats’ countershading characteristics. In many species, countershading was close to the model’s prediction of “optimal” countershading for SSC. Stronger countershading was associated with increased use of open lighting environments, living closer to the equator, and small body size. Abrupt transitions from dark to light tones were more common in open lighting environments but unassociated with group size or antipredator behavior. Though the SSC hypothesis prediction for stronger countershading in diurnal species was not supported and noncountershaded or reverse-countershaded species were unexpectedly common, this basic pattern of associations is explained only by the SSC hypothesis. Despite extreme variation in lighting conditions, many terrestrial animals still find protection from predation by compensating for their own shadows.


Vision Research | 2000

Spatial resolution and receptive field height of motion sensors in human vision

Mark A. Georgeson; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

We estimated the length of motion-detecting receptive fields in human vision by measuring direction discrimination for three novel stimuli. The motion sequences contained either (i) alternate frames of two differently oriented sinusoidal gratings; (ii) alternate frames of vertical grating and plaid stimuli or (iii) a vertical grating divided into horizontal strips of equal height, where alternate strips moved leftward and rightward. All three stimulus sequences had a similar appearance (of moving strips) and the task was to identify the direction of the central strip. For sequences (ii) and (iii), performance fell as the strip height decreased. Threshold height fell with increasing contrast up to about 20%, then levelled off at the critical strip height. Temporal frequency (1. 9-15 Hz) had no effect on the critical strip height. We argue that the receptive field length corresponds to twice this critical height. The length estimates were strikingly short, ranging from about 0.4 cycles at 3.0 cpd to 0.1 cycles at 0.1 cpd. These lengths agree well with the estimates derived at threshold by Anderson and Burr (1991, J. Opt. Soc. Am. A8, 1330-1339), and imply that the motion-sensing filters have very broad orientation tuning. These and other results are interpreted within the framework of a Gaussian derivative model for motion filtering. The sensitivity of motion filters to a broad range of orientations suggests a simpler view of how coherent plaid motion is processed.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 1998

Stereoscopic and contrast-defined motion in human vision

Andrew T. Smith; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

There is considerable evidence for the existence of a specialized mechanism in human vision for detecting moving contrast modulations and some evidence for a mechanism for detecting moving stereoscopic depth modulations. It is unclear whether a single second–order motion mechanism detects both types of stimulus or whether they are detected separately. We show that sensitivity to stereo–defined motion resembles that to contrast–defined motion in two important ways. First, when a missing–fundamental disparity waveform is moved in steps of 0.25 cycles, its perceived direction tends to reverse. This is a property of both luminance–defined and contrast–defined motion and is consistent with independent detection of motion at different spatial scales. Second, thresholds for detecting the direction of a smoothly drifting sinusoidal disparity modulation are much higher than those for detecting its orientation. This is a property of contrast–modulated gratings but not luminance–modulated gratings, for which the two thresholds are normally identical. The results suggest that stereo–defined and contrast–defined motion stimuli are detected either by a common mechanism or by separate mechanisms sharing a common principle of operation.


Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B | 2017

How camouflage works

Sami Merilaita; Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel; Innes C. Cuthill

For camouflage to succeed, an individual has to pass undetected, unrecognized or untargeted, and hence it is the processing of visual information that needs to be deceived. Camouflage is therefore an adaptation to the perception and cognitive mechanisms of another animal. Although this has been acknowledged for a long time, there has been no unitary account of the link between visual perception and camouflage. Viewing camouflage as a suite of adaptations to reduce the signal-to-noise ratio provides the necessary common framework. We review the main processes in visual perception and how animal camouflage exploits these. We connect the function of established camouflage mechanisms to the analysis of primitive features, edges, surfaces, characteristic features and objects (a standard hierarchy of processing in vision science). Compared to the commonly used research approach based on established camouflage mechanisms, we argue that our approach based on perceptual processes targeted by camouflage has several important benefits: specifically, it enables the formulation of more precise hypotheses and addresses questions that cannot even be identified when investigating camouflage only through the classic approach based on the patterns themselves. It also promotes a shift from the appearance to the mechanistic function of animal coloration. This article is part of the themed issue ‘Animal coloration: production, perception, function and application’.

Collaboration


Dive into the Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jian Li

University of Bristol

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge