Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Noam Sobel is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Noam Sobel.


Nature Neuroscience | 2003

Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction

Adam K. Anderson; Kalina Christoff; Iris Stappen; D. Panitz; Dara G. Ghahremani; Gary H. Glover; John D. E. Gabrieli; Noam Sobel

Affective experience has been described in terms of two primary dimensions: intensity and valence. In the human brain, it is intrinsically difficult to dissociate the neural coding of these affective dimensions for visual and auditory stimuli, but such dissociation is more readily achieved in olfaction, where intensity and valence can be manipulated independently. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we found amygdala activation to be associated with intensity, and not valence, of odors. Activity in regions of orbitofrontal cortex, in contrast, were associated with valence independent of intensity. These findings show that distinct olfactory regions subserve the analysis of the degree and quality of olfactory stimulation, suggesting that the affective representations of intensity and valence draw upon dissociable neural substrates.


Nature | 1998

Sniffing and smelling: separate subsystems in the human olfactory cortex

Noam Sobel; Vivek Prabhakaran; John E. Desmond; Gary H. Glover; Richard L. Goode; Edith V. Sullivan; John D. E. Gabrieli

The sensation and perception of smell (olfaction) are largely dependent on sniffing, which is an active stage of stimulus transport and therefore an integral component of mammalian olfaction,. Electrophysiological data obtained from study of the hedgehog, rat, rabbit, dog and monkey indicate that sniffing (whether or not an odorant is present) induces an oscillation of activity in the olfactory bulb, driving the piriform cortex in the temporal lobe, in other words, the piriform is driven by the olfactory bulb at the frequency of sniffing. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that is dependent on the level of oxygen in the blood to determine whether sniffing can induce activation in the piriform of humans, and whether this activation can be differentiated from activation induced by an odorant. We find that sniffing, whether odorant is present or absent, induces activation primarily in the piriform cortex of the temporal lobe and in the medial and posterior orbito-frontal gyri of the frontal lobe. The source of the sniff-induced activation is the somatosensory stimulation that is induced by air flow through the nostrils. In contrast, a smell, regardless of sniffing, induces activation mainly in the lateral and anterior orbito-frontal gyri of the frontal lobe. The dissociation between regions activated by olfactory exploration (sniffing) and regions activated by olfactory content (smell) shows a distinction in brain organization in terms of human olfaction.


The Journal of Neuroscience | 2007

Predicting Odor Pleasantness from Odorant Structure: Pleasantness as a Reflection of the Physical World

Rehan M. Khan; Chung-Hay Luk; Adeen Flinker; Amit Aggarwal; Hadas Lapid; Rafi Haddad; Noam Sobel

Although it is agreed that physicochemical features of molecules determine their perceived odor, the rules governing this relationship remain unknown. A significant obstacle to such understanding is the high dimensionality of features describing both percepts and molecules. We applied a statistical method to reduce dimensionality in both odor percepts and physicochemical descriptors for a large set of molecules. We found that the primary axis of perception was odor pleasantness, and critically, that the primary axis of physicochemical properties reflected the primary axis of olfactory perception. This allowed us to predict the pleasantness of novel molecules by their physicochemical properties alone. Olfactory perception is strongly shaped by experience and learning. However, our findings suggest that olfactory pleasantness is also partially innate, corresponding to a natural axis of maximal discriminability among biologically relevant molecules.


Annual Review of Psychology | 2010

An Odor is Not Worth a Thousand Words: From Multidimensional Odors to Unidimensional Odor Objects

Yaara Yeshurun; Noam Sobel

Olfaction is often referred to as a multidimensional sense. It is multidimensional in that approximately 1000 different receptor types, each tuned to particular odor aspects, together contribute to the olfactory percept. In humans, however, this percept is nearly unidimensional. Humans can detect and discriminate countless odorants, but can identify few by name. The one thing humans can and do invariably say about an odor is whether it is pleasant or not. We argue that this hedonic determination is the key function of olfaction. Thus, the boundaries of an odor object are determined by its pleasantness, which--unlike something material and more like an emotion--remains poorly delineated with words.


Nature Neuroscience | 2007

Mechanisms of scent-tracking in humans.

Jess Porter; Brent A. Craven; Rehan M. Khan; Shao-Ju Chang; Irene Kang; Benjamin Judkewitz; Jason Volpe; Gary S. Settles; Noam Sobel

Whether mammalian scent-tracking is aided by inter-nostril comparisons is unknown. We assessed this in humans and found that (i) humans can scent-track, (ii) they improve with practice, (iii) the human nostrils sample spatially distinct regions separated by ∼3.5 cm and, critically, (iv) scent-tracking is aided by inter-nostril comparisons. These findings reveal fundamental mechanisms of scent-tracking and suggest that the poor reputation of human olfaction may reflect, in part, behavioral demands rather than ultimate abilities.


Nature Neuroscience | 2005

Attentional modulation in human primary olfactory cortex

Christina Zelano; Moustafa Bensafi; Jess Porter; Brad Johnson; Elizabeth A. Bremner; Christina Telles; Rehan M. Khan; Noam Sobel

Central to the concept of attention is the fact that identical stimuli can be processed in different ways. In olfaction, attention may designate the identical flow of air through the nose as either respiration or olfactory exploration. Here we have used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to probe this attentional mechanism in primary olfactory cortex (POC). We report a dissociation in POC that revealed attention-dependent and attention-independent subregions. Whereas a temporal subregion comprising temporal piriform cortex (PirT) responded equally across conditions, a frontal subregion comprising frontal piriform cortex (PirF) and the olfactory tubercle responded preferentially to attended sniffs as opposed to unattended sniffs. In addition, a task-specific anticipatory response occurred in the attention-dependent region only. This dissociation was consistent across two experimental designs: one focusing on sniffs of clean air, the other focusing on odor-laden sniffs. Our findings highlight the role of attention at the earliest cortical levels of olfactory processing.


Nature Methods | 2008

A metric for odorant comparison.

Rafi Haddad; Rehan M. Khan; Yuji Takahashi; Kensaku Mori; David Harel; Noam Sobel

In studies of vision and audition, stimuli can be systematically varied by wavelength and frequency, respectively, but there is no equivalent metric for olfaction. Restricted odorant-feature metrics such as number of carbons and functional group do not account for response patterns to odorants varying along other structural dimensions. We generated a multidimensional odor metric, in which each odorant molecule was represented as a vector of 1,664 molecular descriptor values. Revisiting many studies, we found that this metric and a second optimized metric were always better at accounting for neural responses than the specific metric used in each study. These metrics were applicable across studies that differed in the animals studied, the type of olfactory neurons tested, the odorants applied and the recording methods used. We use this new metric to recommend sets of odorants that span the physicochemical space for use in olfaction experiments.


The Journal of Neuroscience | 2007

Smelling a Single Component of Male Sweat Alters Levels of Cortisol in Women

Claire Wyart; Wallace W. Webster; Jonathan H. Chen; Sarah R. Wilson; Andrew McClary; Rehan M. Khan; Noam Sobel

Rodents use chemosignals to alter endocrine balance in conspecifics. Although responses to human sweat suggest a similar mechanism in humans, no particular component of human sweat capable of altering endocrine balance in conspecifics has yet been isolated and identified. Here, we measured salivary levels of the hormone cortisol in women after smelling pure androstadienone (4,16-androstadien-3-one), a molecule present in the sweat of men that has been suggested as a chemosignal in humans. We found that merely smelling androstadienone maintained significantly higher levels of the hormone cortisol in women. These results suggest that, like rodents, humans can influence the hormonal balance of conspecifics through chemosignals. Critically, this study identified a single component of sweat, androstadienone, as capable of exerting such influence. This result points to a potential role for synthetic human chemosignals in clinical applications.


Nature Neuroscience | 2003

Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception

Moustafa Bensafi; Jessica Porter; Sandra Pouliot; B. R. Johnson; Christina Zelano; Natasha Young; Elizabeth A. Bremner; Danny Aframian; Rehan M. Khan; Noam Sobel

Neural representations created in the absence of external sensory stimuli are referred to as imagery, and such representations may be augmented by reenactment of sensorimotor processes. We measured nasal airflow in human subjects while they imagined sights, sounds and smells, and only during olfactory imagery did subjects spontaneously enact the motor component of olfaction—that is, they sniffed. Moreover, as in perception, imagery of pleasant odors involved larger sniffs than imagery of unpleasant odors, suggesting that the act of sniffing has a functional role in creating of olfactory percepts.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2001

An impairment in sniffing contributes to the olfactory impairment in Parkinson's disease

Noam Sobel; Moriah E. Thomason; Iris Stappen; Caroline M. Tanner; James W. Tetrud; James M. Bower; Edith V. Sullivan; John D. E. Gabrieli

Although the presence of an olfactory impairment in Parkinsons disease (PD) has been recognized for 25 years, its cause remains unclear. Here we suggest a contributing factor to this impairment, namely, that PD impairs active sniffing of odorants. We tested 10 men and 10 women with clinically typical PD, and 20 age- and gender-matched healthy controls, in four olfactory tasks: (i) the University of Pennsylvania smell identification test; (ii and iii) detection threshold tests for the odorants vanillin and propionic acid; and (iv) a two-alternative forced-choice detection paradigm during which sniff parameters (airflow peak rate, mean rate, volume, and duration) were recorded with a pneomatotachograph-coupled spirometer. An additional experiment tested the effect of intentionally increasing sniff vigor on olfactory performance in 20 additional patients. PD patients were significantly impaired in olfactory identification (P < 0.0001) and detection (P < 0.007). As predicted, PD patients were also significantly impaired at sniffing, demonstrating significantly reduced sniff airflow rate (P < 0.01) and volume (P < 0.002). Furthermore, a patients ability to sniff predicted his or her performance on olfactory tasks, i.e., the more poorly patients sniffed, the worse their performance on olfaction tests (P < 0.009). Finally, increasing sniff vigor improved olfactory performance in those patients whose baseline performance had been poorest (P < 0.05). These findings implicate a sniffing impairment as a component of the olfactory impairment in PD and further depict sniffing as an important component of human olfaction.

Collaboration


Dive into the Noam Sobel's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rehan M. Khan

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anat Arzi

Weizmann Institute of Science

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lavi Secundo

Weizmann Institute of Science

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lee Sela

Weizmann Institute of Science

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John D. E. Gabrieli

McGovern Institute for Brain Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Idan Frumin

Weizmann Institute of Science

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Aharon Weissbrod

Weizmann Institute of Science

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anton Plotkin

Ben-Gurion University of the Negev

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge