Nomi Mittelman
Technion – Israel Institute of Technology
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Nomi Mittelman.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2008
Hillel Pratt; Arnold Starr; Henry J. Michalewski; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman
OBJECTIVE The auditory Event-Related Potentials (ERP) of component P50 to sound onset and offset have been reported to be similar, but their magnetic homologue has been reported absent to sound offset. We compared the spatio-temporal distribution of cortical activity during P50 to sound onset and offset, without confounds of spectral change. METHODS ERPs were recorded in response to onsets and offsets of silent intervals of 0.5 s (gaps) appearing randomly in otherwise continuous white noise and compared to ERPs to randomly distributed click pairs with half second separation presented in silence. Subjects were awake and distracted from the stimuli by reading a complicated text. Measures of P50 included peak latency and amplitude, as well as source current density estimates to the clicks and sound onsets and offsets. RESULTS P50 occurred in response to noise onsets and to clicks, while to noise offset it was absent. Latency of P50 was similar to noise onset (56 ms) and to clicks (53 ms). Sources of P50 to noise onsets and clicks included bilateral superior parietal areas. In contrast, noise offsets activated left inferior temporal and occipital areas at the time of P50. Source current density was significantly higher to noise onset than offset in the vicinity of the temporo-parietal junction. CONCLUSIONS P50 to sound offset is absent compared to the distinct P50 to sound onset and to clicks, at different intracranial sources. P50 to stimulus onset and to clicks appears to reflect preattentive arousal by a new sound in the scene. Sound offset does not involve a new sound and hence the absent P50. SIGNIFICANCE Stimulus onset activates distinct early cortical processes that are absent to offset.
Hearing Research | 2010
Hillel Pratt; Arnold Starr; Henry J. Michalewski; Andrew Dimitrijevic; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman
The purpose of this study was to compare cortical brain responses evoked by amplitude modulated acoustic beats of 3 and 6 Hz in tones of 250 and 1000 Hz with those evoked by their binaural beats counterparts in unmodulated tones to indicate whether the cortical processes involved differ. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded to 3- and 6-Hz acoustic and binaural beats in 2000 ms duration 250 and 1000 Hz tones presented with approximately 1 s intervals. Latency, amplitude and source current density estimates of ERP components to beats-evoked oscillations were determined and compared across beat types, beat frequencies and base (carrier) frequencies. All stimuli evoked tone-onset components followed by oscillations corresponding to the beat frequency, and a subsequent tone-offset complex. Beats-evoked oscillations were higher in amplitude in response to acoustic than to binaural beats, to 250 than to 1000 Hz base frequency and to 3 Hz than to 6 Hz beat frequency. Sources of the beats-evoked oscillations across all stimulus conditions located mostly to left temporal lobe areas. Differences between estimated sources of potentials to acoustic and binaural beats were not significant. The perceptions of binaural beats involve cortical activity that is not different than acoustic beats in distribution and in the effects of beat- and base frequency, indicating similar cortical processing.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2009
Hillel Pratt; Arnold Starr; Henry J. Michalewski; Andrew Dimitrijevic; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman
OBJECTIVE To define brain activity corresponding to an auditory illusion of 3 and 6Hz binaural beats in 250Hz or 1000Hz base frequencies, and compare it to the sound onset response. METHODS Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) were recorded in response to unmodulated tones of 250 or 1000Hz to one ear and 3 or 6Hz higher to the other, creating an illusion of amplitude modulations (beats) of 3Hz and 6Hz, in base frequencies of 250Hz and 1000Hz. Tones were 2000ms in duration and presented with approximately 1s intervals. Latency, amplitude and source current density estimates of ERP components to tone onset and subsequent beats-evoked oscillations were determined and compared across beat frequencies with both base frequencies. RESULTS All stimuli evoked tone-onset P(50), N(100) and P(200) components followed by oscillations corresponding to the beat frequency, and a subsequent tone-offset complex. Beats-evoked oscillations were higher in amplitude with the low base frequency and to the low beat frequency. Sources of the beats-evoked oscillations across all stimulus conditions located mostly to left lateral and inferior temporal lobe areas in all stimulus conditions. Onset-evoked components were not different across stimulus conditions; P(50) had significantly different sources than the beats-evoked oscillations; and N(100) and P(200) sources located to the same temporal lobe regions as beats-evoked oscillations, but were bilateral and also included frontal and parietal contributions. CONCLUSIONS Neural activity with slightly different volley frequencies from left and right ear converges and interacts in the central auditory brainstem pathways to generate beats of neural activity to modulate activities in the left temporal lobe, giving rise to the illusion of binaural beats. Cortical potentials recorded to binaural beats are distinct from onset responses. SIGNIFICANCE Brain activity corresponding to an auditory illusion of low frequency beats can be recorded from the scalp.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2009
Hillel Pratt; Arnold Starr; Henry J. Michalewski; Andrew Dimitrijevic; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman
OBJECTIVE To define cortical brain responses to large and small frequency changes (increase and decrease) of high- and low-frequency tones. METHODS Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) were recorded in response to a 10% or a 50% frequency increase from 250 or 4000 Hz tones that were approximately 3 s in duration and presented at 500-ms intervals. Frequency increase was followed after 1 s by a decrease back to base frequency. Frequency changes occurred at least 1 s before or after tone onset or offset, respectively. Subjects were not attending to the stimuli. Latency, amplitude and source current density estimates of ERPs were compared across frequency changes. RESULTS All frequency changes evoked components P(50), N(100), and P(200). N(100) and P(200) had double peaks at bilateral and right temporal sites, respectively. These components were followed by a slow negativity (SN). The constituents of N(100) were predominantly localized to temporo-parietal auditory areas. The potentials and their intracranial distributions were affected by both base frequency (larger potentials to low frequency) and direction of change (larger potentials to increase than decrease), as well as by change magnitude (larger potentials to larger change). The differences between frequency increase and decrease depended on base frequency (smaller difference to high frequency) and were localized to frontal areas. CONCLUSIONS Brain activity varies according to frequency change direction and magnitude as well as base frequency. SIGNIFICANCE The effects of base frequency and direction of change may reflect brain networks involved in more complex processing such as speech that are differentially sensitive to frequency modulations of high (consonant discrimination) and low (vowels and prosody) frequencies.
Journal of the Neurological Sciences | 1995
Bernard M. Solliway; Alex Schaffer; A. Erez; Nomi Mittelman; Hillel Pratt; Shmuel Yannai
Visual event-related potentials were measured in lead-exposed and control subjects, while they were performing a target detection as well as a memory scanning task. Blood lead and urinary delta-aminolevulinic acid (delta-ALA) were determined in samples taken on the same day. Lead exposure did not affect the memory scanning P300 latency, but it did delay the target detection P300 latency in a dose-dependent fashion. The P300 amplitude of lead-exposed subjects was significantly reduced for both tasks, but not in a dose-dependent fashion. The target detection, but not the memory scanning, P300 latency correlated with urinary delta-ALA. No correlation of P300 with age was found, even though the subjects ranged from 20 to 60 years of age. The difference in the effect of lead exposure on the target detection and memory scanning P300 adds to the evidence that the P300 for the two tasks arises from different generators. The absence of a correlation of the measured P300 latency for each task with age in the present study raises the possibility that this extensively reported observation might, in part, be due to inappropriately matched younger and older subjects. This study indicates that evaluation of subjects exposed to toxic substances can increase our basic understanding of evoked potentials, as well as providing evidence of their toxic manifestations.
Neurophysiologie Clinique-clinical Neurophysiology | 2000
Hillel Pratt; N Karim; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman
OBJECTIVES Short latency visual evoked potentials (SVEP), in response to high-intensity flashes from light emitting diodes (LED), were used to detect subclinical effects along the visual pathway in four groups of subjects with different levels of exposure to gasoline, all within legally acceptable limits. METHODS Potentials and exposure levels were obtained from 31 subjects with different occupational exposure levels to gasoline fumes, as well as from 17 non-exposed control subjects. SVEP were recorded from four electrode sites (infra-orbital, Cz, Pz, Oz), in response to flashes presented to each eye in turn from goggle-mounted LEDs. SVEP components were defined after digital filtering, which eliminated the high-frequency oscillatory potentials and accentuated five major components: a periocular P30, attributed to the retina; a fronto-central N50, attributed to the optic nerve; centro-parietal P65 and N85, attributed to the optic tracts and radiation; and an occipital, cortical P105. RESULTS The latencies of successive SVEP components of the exposed subjects showed a significant latency prolongation compared to controls, beginning with activity attributed to the optic nerve and increasing cumulatively with the later components. Retinal components were not affected by the exposure to organic solvents. Among the exposed groups, differences in latency prolongation corresponded to occupational exposure. CONCLUSION The low-frequency components of SVEP were reliably measured and proved to be sensitive to subclinical effects of organic solvents on conduction along the visual pathway. These components are likely to be sensitive to other subcortical visual pathway lesions, but their clinical promise needs further verification.
Brain and behavior | 2015
Hillel Pratt; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman
Spatio‐temporal distributions of cortical activity to audio‐visual presentations of meaningless vowel‐consonant‐vowels and the effects of audio‐visual congruence/incongruence, with emphasis on the McGurk effect, were studied. The McGurk effect occurs when a clearly audible syllable with one consonant, is presented simultaneously with a visual presentation of a face articulating a syllable with a different consonant and the resulting percept is a syllable with a consonant other than the auditorily presented one.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2004
Hillel Pratt; Nomi Mittelman; Naomi Bleich; Ilan Laufer
OBJECTIVE The purpose of this study was to define early brain activity associated with fusion of speech elements to form an auditory object in the middle-latency range preceding the F-Complex. METHODS Stimuli were binaural formant transition and base, that were presented separately or fused to form the vowel-consonant-vowel sequence /ada/. Eleven right-handed, adult, native Hebrew speakers listened to 2/s presentations, and the brain potentials from C(z) during the 250 msec following transition onset (in the responses to transition and to the fused word) or following the time it would have been presented (in the response to base alone) were recorded. The net-fusion response was extracted by subtracting the sum of potentials to the base and the formant transition from the potentials to the fused sound. RESULTS Auditory middle-latency components, comprising of 9 peaks and troughs were recorded in response to the base, to the formant transition and to the fused /ada/. In general, the responses to the fused object were significantly smaller in peak amplitude and in total activity (area under the curve) resulting in the difference waveform of the net-fusion response that also included 9 peaks, but with opposite polarities. CONCLUSIONS The early middle-latency components to fusion indicate that the fusion of speech elements to a word involves inhibition, occlusion or both. The results are in line with the uniqueness of speech perception and the early role of the auditory cortex in speech analysis.
Human Brain Mapping | 2013
Hillel Pratt; Dalal Abu-Amneh Abbasi; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman; Arnold Starr
This study determined the effects of phonology and semantics on the distribution of cortical activity to the second of a pair of words in first and second language (mixed pairs). The effects of relative proficiency in the two languages and linguistic setting (monolinguistic or mixed) are reported in a companion paper. Ten early bilinguals and 14 late bilinguals listened to mixed pairs of words in Arabic (L1) and Hebrew (L2) and indicated whether both words in the pair had the same or different meanings. The spatio‐temporal distribution of current densities of event‐related potentials were estimated for each language and according to semantic and phonologic relationship (same or different) compared with the first word in the pair. During early processing (<300 ms), brain activity in temporal and temporoparietal auditory areas was enhanced by phonologic incongruence between words in the pair and in Wernickes area by both phonologic and semantic priming. In contrast, brain activities during late processing (>300 ms) were enhanced by semantic incongruence between the two words, particularly in temporal areas and in left hemisphere Brocas and Wernickes areas. The latter differences were greater when words were in L2. Surprisingly, no significant effects of relative proficiency on processing the second word in the pair were found. These results indicate that the distribution of brain activity to the second of two words presented bilingually is affected differently during early and late processing by both semantic and phonologic priming by‐ and incongruence with the immediately preceding word. Hum Brain Mapp 34:2882–2898, 2013.
Human Brain Mapping | 2013
Hillel Pratt; Dalal Abu-Amneh Abbasi; Naomi Bleich; Nomi Mittelman; Arnold Starr
The study determined how spatiotemporal distribution of cortical activity to words in first and second language is affected by language, proficiency, and linguistic setting. Ten early bilinguals and 14 late adult bilinguals listened to pairs of words presented in Arabic (L1), Hebrew (L2), or in mixed pairs and indicated whether both words had the same meaning or not. Source current densities of event‐related potentials were estimated. Activity to first words in the pair lateralized to right hemisphere, higher to L1 than L2 during early processing (<300 ms) among both groups but only among late bilinguals during late processing (>300 ms). During early and late processing, activities were larger in mixed than monolinguistic settings among early bilinguals but lower in mixed than in monolinguistic settings among late bilinguals. Late processing in auditory regions was of larger magnitude in left than right hemispheres among both groups. Activity to second words in the pair was larger in mixed than in monolinguistic settings during both early and late processing among both groups. Early processing of second words in auditory regions lateralized to the right among early bilinguals and to the left among late bilinguals, whereas late processing did not differ between groups. Wernickes area activity during late processing of L2 was larger on the right, while on the left no significant differences between languages were found. The results show that cortical language processing in bilinguals differs between early and late processing and these differences are modulated by linguistic proficiency and setting. Hum Brain Mapp 34:2863–2881, 2013.