Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Noriko Hattori is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Noriko Hattori.


Experimental Brain Research | 2010

An event-related fNIRS investigation of Japanese word order

Yukika Nishimura; Koji Sugisaki; Noriko Hattori; Yasushi Inokuchi; Masayuki Komachi; Yoshihiro Nishimura; Mariko Ogawa; Motohiro Okada; Yuji Okazaki; Waro Taki; Tetsuro Yamamoto; Etsuko Yoshida; Seiki Ayano

Japanese is a free word-order language, and allows both subject–object–verb (SOV) and object–subject–verb (OSV) orders. Our previous study using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) imaging revealed that OSV sentences induce more activation in the left frontal lobe than SOV sentences. The present study develops our previous experiment: (1) by adopting an event-related design, and (2) by using sentences involving the adverb naze ‘why’, which plays a prominent role in recent linguistic studies. The results of our new experiment indicated that the cerebral activation in OwhySV sentences was significantly larger than that in SwhyOV sentences, in the right anterior prefrontal region, which is consistent with the assumption that OwhySV order is derived from SwhyOV order. We speculate that the activation observed in the anterior prefrontal cortex during the processing of the sentences involving ‘why’ might be due to the processing of higher-order function in the cerebral cortex.


Neuroreport | 2008

A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study on the basic word order in Japanese

Yukika Nishimura; Koji Sugisaki; Noriko Hattori; Yasushi Inokuchi; Yoshihiro Nishimura; Mariko Ogawa; Motohiro Okada; Yuji Okazaki; Waro Taki; Tetsuro Yamamoto; Etsuko Yoshida; Seiki Ayano

To determine the basic word order in Japanese, oxyhemoglobin concentration changes in the frontal region of 32 healthy men were measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Our experiment used four types of sentences: (i) subject-object-verb (SOV), (ii) object-subject-verb (OSV), (iii) subject-naze (why)-object-verb (SwhyOV), and (iv) naze (why)-subject-object-verb (whySOV). The results have shown that although oxyhemoglobin changes in the OSV sentences were significantly larger than those in the SOV sentences in the left frontal lobe, such effects were not observed between SwhyOV sentences and whySOV sentences. These results are consistent with the view that both SwhyOV and whySOV are the basic, which suggests that neurolinguistic evidence has the potential to provide an important basis for determining the basic word order in a free word-order language.


Neuroscience Research | 2009

Cortical activation by syntactic violations in Japanese: an fNIRS study

Seiki Ayano; Yukika Nishimura; Koji Sugisaki; Noriko Hattori; Yasushi Inokuchi; Shozo Kojima; Masayuki Komachi; Yoshihiro Nishimura; Yukio Otsu; Mariko Ogawa; Motohiro Okada; Satoshi Umeda; Tetsuro Yamamoto; Etsuko Yoshida

To investigate the neural correlates of proficiency in second language, we analyzed T1-weighted 3-dimensional structural MRIs obtained from 27 healthy, dextral, native Japanese who had learnt English as a second language. The English Vocabulary Test (EVT) was employed as a representative measure of proficiency in English. The gray matter was segmented from each structural MRI, spatially normalized to fit into a standard gray-matter template, and smoothed with a 12-mm Gaussian kernel (SPM5). A voxel-based morphometry analysis incorporated EVT scores, age and sex. The EVT score was correlated with gray matter volumes in the bilateral inferior frontal gyri, bilateral caudate nuclei, right middle frontal gyrus, and left middle temporal gyrus (p < 0.001 uncorrected, voxel size >150). Structural changes in a set of cortical and subcortical regions might underlie proficiency in second language.


Neuroscience Research | 2007

Flexible word order in Japanese and its effects on frontal activation: A NIRS study

Yukika Nishimura; Koji Sugisaki; Noriko Hattori; Yasushi Inokuchi; Mariko Ogawa; Yuji Okazaki; Waro Taki; Tetsuro Yamamoto; Etsuko Yoshida; Seiki Ayano

s / Neuroscience Research 58S (2007) S1–S244 S173 P2-h20 Behavioral responses to frequency modulated sounds in Mongolian gerbils: Upward FM versus downward FM Yuki Nagata Department of knowledge Engineering and Computer Sciences, Doshisha University, Kyoto, Japan It is known that Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguicultus) have communication calls that are mainly frequency modulated (FM) sounds. They vocalize downward FM (dFM) sounds more often than upward FM (uFM) sounds in low frequency range (<16 kHz). We considered that gerbils could perceive small differences between the dFM rates, since they use different types of dFM sounds in daily life. We compared behavioral responses to dFM sounds with those to uFM sounds by using GO/NOGO discrimination tasks. Gerbils were trained to discriminate FM sounds (dFM sound and uFM sound) from constant frequency (CF) sounds. After training was completed, test stimuli with different FM rates were presented to gerbils. As a result, the go response decreased as the FM rate decreased in the cace of dFM sounds. However, gerbils did not appear to discriminate differences within uFM sounds. Research fund: Grant to RCAST at Doshisha University from MEXT, Innovative Cluster Creation Project by MEXT P2-h21 Phonological and lexico-semantic processing during kanji word reading: An MEG study Qiang Wei1,2, Aya Ihara1, Tomoe Hayakawa1,3, Tsutomu Murata1, Norio Fujimaki1,2 1 Biological ICT Group, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Kobe, Japan; 2 Department of Brain Science and Engineering, Kyushu Institute of Technology, Kitakyushu, Japan; 3 Department of Psychology, Teikyo University, Tokyo, Japan We measured neural activities including the effect of phonological information on the lexico-semantic process during silent reading of kanji homophone words by using MEG. Subjects were presented prime-target word pairs that consisted of two kanji characters. The primes were phonologically same as or different from the following target words, or pseudo-characters. The neural activation differed between conditions in the left posterior superior temporal and inferior parietal areas, and became weaker by the phonological repetition. Furthermore, activity was larger for different condition in the anterior temporal lobe, and for same condition in Broca’s area, as compared with the other two conditions at 400–500 ms. Our results suggest that the phonological information influences the lexico-semantic process even under the strong constraint of orthographic information. P2-h22 Two distinct neural networks for semantic access during visual word recognition Hyeonjeong Jeong1, Motoaki Sugiura1,2, Yuko Sassa1,4, Tadao Miyamoto3, Kaoru Horie3, Shigeru Sato3, Ryuta Kawashima1 1 Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan; 2 Department of Cortical Research, NIPS, Japan; 3 Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University, Japan; 4 RISTEX, JST, Japan To investigate how Japanese efficiently access the meanings of written words in the brain, this study employed a two-by-two factorial design that manipulates both word types (Kanji vs. Kana) and task types (semantic vs. phonological). Two main findings emerged. First, differential semantic – phonological activation was significantly greater for Kanji than Kana in the left inferior temporal gyrus, left parahippocampal gyrus, left hippocampus, left caudate and right anterior fusiform gyrus. This finding suggests that these areas constitute the direct pathway from orthography to semantic system. Second, differential semantic – phonological activation was greater for Kana than Kanji in the left inferior occipital gyrus. This indicates that these areas are involved in reading through phonological mediation prior to accessing the semantic system. P2-h23 IFG activity in observing music performance Masumi Wakita1,2 1 Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Aichi, Japan; 2 Japan Science Technology Agency, Japan Broca’s area is involved in processing of hierarchical sensory and motor structures, and is activated when subjects are observing practiced actions. The current study was conducted on the hypothesis that the response of this region while viewing a structured action, such as music performance, would be influenced by the experience of observers. To this end, healthy right-handed adult participants were exposed to hand movements playing either ‘Twinkle, twinkle, little star’ or ‘Mary had a little lamb’ that was paired with its piano sounds either in a congruent or in an incongruent fashion. The subjects were asked to determine which music the hand was performing independent of the heard sounds. Cortical activity was measured using a near-infrared spectroscopy. Consequently, Broca’s area of the expert subjects showed higher activity in the incongruent condition requiring a higher processing load although all the subjects answered the song names correctly. Thus, Broca’s area is assumed to be involved in transforming a sequence of discrete actions into unified meaningful performance. Research fund: 21c COE (A14), JST, Nakayama Fundation for Human Science P2-h24 Early GABA function regulates sensory critical period during birdsong learning Yoko Yazaki-Sugiyama1,2, Jason Kushner3, Neal A Hessler2,3, Takao K Hensch1,2 1 CREST, JST, Osaka, Japan; 2 Neuronal Circuit Dev, RIKEN BSI, Wako, Japan; 3 Vocal Behabior Mech, RIKEN BSI, Wako, Japan Neural circuits are shaped by experience during restricted critical periods in early life. In mouse visual cortex, this timing is triggered by the maturation of inhibitory function. Here, we tested whether the principle also applies to another widely studied system, birdsong learning. Male zebra finches were injected into the ventricle with the positive GABAA receptor modulator diazepam (DZ) or vehicle control for 4 consecutive days before the critical period (∼20 days post-hatch). Birds in which inhibitory function was briefly accelerated suffered long-lasting learning deficits (>120 days) across several parameters: delayed singing onset, reduced song consistency or similarity to the tutors’ songs. Rate of motor learning was not affected. Premature closure of the sensory acquisition phase was instead implicated using a sequential, dual-tutoring paradigm, as well as by neuroanatomical consequences of early DZ treatment. Taken together, inhibitory circuits may generally influence the timing of developmental critical periods. Research fund: CREST P2-h25 Flexible word order in Japanese and its effects on frontal activation: A NIRS study Y. Nishimura1, K. Sugisaki2, N. Hattori2, Y. Inokuchi2, Y. Nishimura1, M. Ogawa2, Y. Okazaki1,3, W. Taki1, T. Yamamoto1, E. Yoshida2, Seiki Ayano2 1 Division of Neuroscience, Mie University Graduate School of Medicine, Mie, Japan; 2 Faculty of Humanity and Social Sciences, Mie University, Mie, Japan; 3 Tokyo Metropolitan Matsuzawa Hospital, Tokyo, Japan Japanese has flexible word order. In order to determine the basic word order in Japanese, oxy-hemoglobin concentration (oxyHb) changes in the frontal region were monitored by a 52-channel near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) in 32 right-handed healthy males during Subject (whyS/Swhy) and Object (SOV/OSV) Scrambling Conditions. The results have shown that while oxyHb changes in the sentences with Object Scrambling Condition (OSV) were significantly larger than that in the non-scrambled sentences (SOV) in the left frontal lobe, such effects cannot be observed in the case of Subject Scrambling Condition. These findings suggest that while OSV is derived from the basic order of SOV, there is no such derivational relationship between whyS and Swhy: both of these constitute the basic order. Research fund: This research was supported in part by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (#17320062).


Language Variation and Change | 1998

Base Transparency in Suprasegmental Changes: Ongoing Changes in Japanese and English.

Noriko Hattori

This article deals with suprasegmental changes in progress in Japanese and English and demonstrates that the same kind of mechanism is in operation. Data on realtime changes in pitch accent, collected by comparing earlier editions of Japanese pronouncing dictionaries with recent editions, are examined. Analysis of data from present-day speech provides supplementary data on innovative accent. Two types of principle for accent change are proposed: the demand for paradigm coherence and the demand for canonical accent pattern. In the case of nouns derived from adjectives, the two appear to be in conflict. It is suggested that in Japanese the demand for paradigm coherence is the stronger principle. Similar kinds of conflict between two possible types of change are observed in ongoing stress changes in English. It is shown that base (or stem) transparency is a major factor in determining the directionality of accent change.


Philologia | 2015

Apparent Exceptions to the Syllabic Distribution Algorithm for English

Noriko Hattori


ICPhS | 2015

Accent and beat matching: The correspondence of English stress and Japanese pitch in terms of textsetting.

Noriko Hattori


Philologia | 2013

Textsetting in English Vocal Music (坂本つや子教授退職記念号)

Noriko Hattori


ICPhS | 2011

Language Discrimination Using Low-pass Filtered Songs: Perception of Different Rhythm Classes.

Hajime Takeyasu; Noriko Hattori


Philologia | 2010

Experiencing English rhythm through note values

Noriko Hattori

Collaboration


Dive into the Noriko Hattori's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge