Padraig G. O'Seaghdha
Lehigh University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Padraig G. O'Seaghdha.
Cognition | 1992
Gary S. Dell; Padraig G. O'Seaghdha
We describe two primary stages in the top-down process of lexical access in production, a stage of lemma access in which words are retrieved as syntactic-semantic entities, and a stage of phonological access in which the forms of the words are fleshed out. We suggest a reconciliation of modular and interactive accounts of these stages whereby modularity is traceable to the action of discrete linguistic rule systems, but interaction arises in the lexical network on which these rules operate. We also discuss the time-course of lexical access in multi-word utterances. We report some initial production priming explorations that support the hypothesis that lemmas are buffered in longer utterances before they are phonologically specified. Because such techniques provide a relatively direct way of assessing activation at the primary stages of lexical access they are an important new resource for the study of language production.
Psychological Review | 1991
Gary S. Dell; Padraig G. O'Seaghdha
Levelt et al. (1991) argued that modular semantic and phonological stage theories of lexical access in language production are to be preferred over interactive spreading-activation theories (e.g., Dell, 1986). As evidence, they show no mediated semantic-phonological priming during picture naming: Retrieval of sheep primes goat, but the activation of goat is not transmitted to its phonological relative, goal. This research reconciles this result with spreading-activation theories and shows how the absence of mediated priming coexists with the convergent priming necessary to account for mixed semantic-phonological speech errors. The analysis leads to the proposal that the language-production system may best be characterized as globally modular but locally interactive.
Cognition | 2010
Padraig G. O'Seaghdha; Jenn Yeu Chen; Train Min Chen
In Mandarin Chinese, speakers benefit from fore-knowledge of what the first syllable but not of what the first phonemic segment of a disyllabic word will be (Chen, Chen, & Dell, 2002), contrasting with findings in English, Dutch, and other Indo-European languages, and challenging the generality of current theories of word production. In this article, we extend the evidence for the language difference by showing that failure to prepare onsets in Mandarin (Experiment 1) applies even to simple monosyllables (Experiments 2-4), and confirm the contrast with English for comparable materials (Experiments 5 and 6). We also provide new evidence that Mandarin speakers do reliably prepare tonally unspecified phonological syllables (Experiment 7). To account for these patterns, we propose a language general proximate units principle whereby intentional preparation for speech as well as phonological-lexical coordination are grounded at the first phonological level below the word at which explicit unit selection occurs. The language difference arises because syllables are proximate units in Mandarin Chinese, whereas segments are proximate in English and other Indo-European languages. The proximate units perspective reconciles the aspiration toward a language general account of word production with the reality of substantial cross-linguistic differences.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2000
Padraig G. O'Seaghdha; Joseph W. Marin
Phonological competition theory states that competition among discrepant segments of similar words leads to inhibition of high-frequency word-naming responses in form-related priming tasks. If segments are selected sequentially, competition should be greater for begin-related pairs (storage-story), in which discrepant segments are late in the words, than for end-related pairs (glory-story), in which discrepant segments are selected before the shared ones. This pattern was not observed in standard visual priming, probably because of the influence of parallel orthographic input. However, it was observed in a repetitive word-pair production task in which visual input was absent. The findings favor a class of models in which nonsequential activation of phonological content precedes sequential selection of the segments of words to be spoken.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 1997
Padraig G. O'Seaghdha
Manipulating the semantic relatedness of noun and verb targets in contexts where they are grammatically appropriate or inappropriate allows for simultaneous examination of syntactic and semantic context effects. A lexical-decision experiment showed both a syntactic context effect and a semantic relatedness effect that was stronger in syntactically appropriate conditions. Thus, latencies appeared to be conjointly determined by syntactic and semantic context. In contrast, naming experiments also showed both semantic and syntactic effects, but the syntactic context effect was independent of semantic relatedness and was observed in the virtual absence of sensitivity to semantic anomaly. Thus, syntactic and semantic processing are largely dissociable in the naming task. In conjunction with other findings in the literature, this suggests the existence of an isolable level of syntactic assignment that precedes semantic integration of content words in sentence comprehension.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2013
Padraig G. O'Seaghdha; Jenn Yeu Chen; Train Min Chen
Converging evidence points to a difference between European and Chinese languages in the type of the initial units of phonological encoding for speaking. The phonological access points or “proximate units” (1, 2) are segmental in Indo-European languages but whole syllables in Chinese. Accordingly, Chinese speakers, unlike English speakers, do not register the presence of consistent initial consonants in several word production tasks. Qu et al.’s (3) intriguing report both supports and challenges this interpretation. In their experiment, Mandarin speaking participants produced picture descriptions comprising a color-adjective and noun that shared or did not share initial segments (e.g., green guitar vs. blue guitar in English). Consistent with previous findings, there was no response time benefit of shared initial phonemes. In seeming contrast, there was an early differentiation between shared and different onset conditions in event-related potentials (ERPs).
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition | 2012
Jenn Yeu Chen; Jui Ju Su; Chao Yang Lee; Padraig G. O'Seaghdha
Chinese and English speakers seem to hold different conceptions of time which may be related to the different codings of time in the two languages. Employing a sentence–picture matching task, we have investigated this linguistic relativity in Chinese–English bilinguals varying in English proficiency and found that those with high proficiency performed differently from those with low proficiency. Additional monolingual English data, reported here, showed further that high-proficiency bilinguals performed similarly to the English monolinguals, suggesting that Chinese speakers’ sensitivity to the time of an action event might be modifiable according to the extent of their experience with a tensed language.
Journal of Memory and Language | 1998
Lynne Stallings; Maryellen C. MacDonald; Padraig G. O'Seaghdha
Journal of Memory and Language | 1997
Padraig G. O'Seaghdha; Joseph W. Marin
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research | 2007
Elvira Perez; Julio Santiago; Alfonso Palma; Padraig G. O'Seaghdha